See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/16053310
Interparental Conflict and Children of Discord and Divorce
ArticleinPsychological Bulletin · October 1982
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.92.2.310·Source: PubMed
CITATIONS
1,125
READS
11,779
1 author:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Child effects View project
Robert Emery
University of Virginia
191 PUBLICATIONS10,321 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Robert Emery on 14 September 2015.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
Psychological
Bulletin
1982,
Vol.
92, No. 2,
310-330
Copyright
1982
by the
American Psychological Association, Inc.
0033-2909/82/9202-0310S00.75
Interparental Conflict
and
the
Children
of
Discord
and
Divorce
Robert
E.
Emery
University
of
Virginia
Data
on the
relation between marital turmoil (i.e., discord
and
divorce)
and
behavior
problems
in
children
are
reviewed.
It is
concluded that
a
relation
be-
tween
the two
domains docs exist. Several parameters
of
this relation
are
outlined,
including
type
of
marital turmoil, form
of the
child's behavioral response,
sex
differences,
age
effects, parental
buffering,
and
effects
of
parental psychopa-
thology.
Conclusions drawn
from
this review
are
used
to
evaluate several broad
etiological
hypotheses about
the
effect
of
marital turmoil
on
children,
and im-
plications
for the
treatment
of
behavior problems
in
children
from
these families
are
discussed. Finally, interpretative
and
methodological refinements
are
sug-
gested
for
future research.
The
idea that marital
turmoil
1
is the
cause
of
a
variety
of
behavior problems
in
children
is
widely held both
in the
public
and in the
professional
domain. During
the
first
6
months
of
1980, such prominent media
as
Newsweek
and the New
York
Times
Mag-
azine
ran
cover stories
on
children
of
divorce,
and
the
National Institute
of
Mental Health
solicited research proposals
on the
topic.
Recent interest
has
been spurred
by
popular
movies
and
books, which themselves reflect
the
turmoil
found
in an
increasing number
of
American families.
In
fact,
the
Academy
Award
winner
for
best motion picture
in
1980
depicted
the
problems
of
marital sep-
aration
and
child custody
(Kramer
vs.
Kra-
mer),
and the
1981
winner portrayed family
conflict
in a
two-parent household (Ordinary
People).
It has
been estimated
that
38% of the first
marriages
of
women
in
their
late
twenties
will
end in
divorce
(Glick
&
Norton, 1978).
Although
divorce
is
slightly less frequent
among
couples
who
have children under
5
years
of
age,
an
estimated
45% of the
chil-
dren born
in
1977
will
live
in a
one-parent
family
for at
least
several months, primarily
as a
result
of
divorce
(Cherlin,
1977; Glick
&
Norton,
1978).
The
census bureau
has
reported
a 79%
increase
in the
number
of
single-parent families between 1970
and
1980,
so the
current prevalence
is one in five
(U.S. Census Bureau, 1980). Because
not
every
discordant marriage
is
dissolved, these
astronomical
figures on the
number
of
chil-
dren
of
divorce omit
a
significant
and
largely
unknown
number
of
children
who are ex-
posed
to
serious marital conflict.
Psychologists have been called
on to
pro-
vide
both explanations
and
treatments
for
families
who are
affected
by
marital turmoil.
The
response
has
encompassed almost
the
entire range
of
possibilities.
Some
psychol-
ogists have suggested that whenever there
is
a
problem child, there
is a
problem mar-
riage
(Framo,
1975); others have concluded
that
a
relation between divorce
and
child
problems
has yet to be
demonstrated
(Her-
zog
&
Sudia, 1973).
The
relative lack
of
sophisticated
research regarding
the
relation
between
marital
turmoil
and
child
problems
encourages
such
a
diversity
of
opinion. Nev-
I
would like
to
thank
K.
Daniel
O'Leary,
Alan
O.
Ross,
and
Everett Waters,
as
well
as
John Darley,
as-
sociate editor,
and
several anonymous reviewers
for
their
comments
on
earlier versions
of
this article.
Requests
for
reprints should
be
sent
to
Robert Emery,
Department
of
Psychology,
Gilmer
Hall,
University
of
Virginia,
Charlottesville,
Virginia 22901.
1
The
term
turmoil
refers
to
families
characterized
by
discord
in
marriage, separation,
or
divorce
as a
group.
The
term
marital
problems
is
occasionally used
as a
synonym.
Interparental conflict
is
used
to
denote open
hostility
between married, separated,
or
divorced par-
ents.
Marital
discord
refers
to
problems
in
intact mar-
riages
only.
310
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
311
ertheless,
careful examination
of the
litera-
ture reveals that
a
considerable body
of in-
formation
is
available.
Covariation Between Marital
and
Child
Problems
A
number
of
investigators have
found
a
relation between discord
in
intact marriages
and the
severity
or
frequency
of
behavior
problems
in
children.
This
rinding
remains
consistent
across such countries
as the
United
States
(Emery
&
O'Leary,
in
press;
Olt-
manns,
Broderick,
&
O'Leary,
1977;
Porter
&
O'Leary,
1980), England (Rutter, 1971,
1979),
and
India (Chawla
&
Gupt, 1979).
Similarly, divorce
and
child problems have
often
been
found
to be
related (Anthony,
1974; Hetherington, 1979;
McDermott,
1968,
1970; Wallerstein
&
Kelly,
1980;
Westman,
Cline,
Swift,
&
Kramer, 1970).
Many
studies
of
marital
and
child
problems,
however,
suffer
from
one or
more
of a
variety
of
methodological
flaws. The
three most
common
problems
are (a)
biased
sampling
usually
from
a
clinic population;
(b)
non-
independent
data—that
is, the
same judges
rate both
the
marriage
and the
child,
or,
in
the
case
of
divorce,
the
judges
of
child
be-
havior
are
aware
of the
marital status;
and
(c) the use of
measures lacking
in
reliability
and
validity.
Each
of
these problems
may
affect
estimates
of the
magnitude
of the as-
sociation between marital
and
child prob-
lems.
Stronger
associations between marital dis-
cord
and
child adjustment seem
to be
found
in
clinic
rather
than
in
nonclinic samples
(O'Leary
&
Emery,
in
press).
In a
series
of
studies
by the
Stony Brook group,
all
of
which used
parents'
ratings
on
well-estab-
lished
measures
of
child behavior (Behavior
Problem
Checklist,
Quay
&
Peterson,
Note
1)
and
marital
adjustment
(Short
Marital
Adjustment
Test,
Locke
&
Wallace, 1959),
stronger associations were
found
in
clinic
(Emery
&
O'Leary,
in
press; Oltmanns
et
al,
1977; Porter
&
O'Leary,
1980) than
in
nonclinic
samples (Oltmanns
et
al.,
1977;
Emery,
Note
2).
Significant relations were
found
in
both sets
of
samples;
the
clinic sam-
ples,
however,
may
have differed from
the
nonclinic
ones
on
some
third
variable
that
increased estimates
of the
association
be-
tween marital
and
child problems
(O'Leary
&
Emery,
in
press).
Some
data
suggest
that
the
expectation
bias
2
(Rosenthal,
1966)
of
raters
of
child
behavior
who are not
blind
to the
marital
relationship
can
also lead
to
higher estimates
of
the
relation between marital
and
child
problems. Robinson
and
Anderson
(Note
3)
found
that
a
significant correlation between
mothers'
checklist
ratings
of
their
marriage
and
their children's adjustment
was
reduced
to a
chance level when social desirability
was
partialed out. Emery
(Note
2)
found stron-
ger
correlations between mothers' marital
ratings
and
their
own
judgments
of
distur-
bance
in
their children than between moth-
ers' marital ratings
and
teachers' child
ad-
justment
scores.
Because
mothers
and
teach-
ers
rate behavior
in
different
settings,
however,
situation-specific behavior could
explain
this
result.
In
fact,
one
might
expect
child
behavior
to be
most
affected
in the
set-
ting
where
the
marital turmoil
is
present.
Thus, independent measures
of
marital dis-
cord
and
child problems
in the
home setting
may be
exceedingly
difficult,
but
important,
to
obtain.
Unlike
the
effects
of
using
clinic samples
and
nonindependent
ratings,
the use of un-
reliable measures would tend
to
attenuate
rather than increase estimates
of the
mag-
nitude
of the
association
between
marital
and
child problems. Researchers
are not
fully
satisfied
with
the
reliability
and
validity
of
the
available
measures
of
either childhood
adjustment
(Achenbach, 1978)
or
marital
relations
(O'Leary
&
Turkewitz, 1978).
It
is
not
surprising,
then, that many
of the re-
ports
reviewed below used measures
of
ques-
2
The
expectation—held
by
parents,
teachers,
and
mental health
professionals—that
marital
turmoil
causes
behavior
problems
in
children
not
only
may
create
biased
rating data
but
also
may be
problematic
in
that
a
self-fulfilling
prophecy
may
result.
For
example, par-
ents
who
attribute
a
child's (normal) misbehavior
to an
emotional
reaction
to
marital turmoil
may not
respond
to
that
child
with
their usual discipline. Thus,
in
their
attempt
to
understand
the
child, some parents
may set
limits
that
are
inconsistent
and
confusing
and
thereby
accidentally
create
the
problems that they
are
trying
to
avoid.
312
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
tionable adequacy. Unreliability
is
especially
important
to
consider when comparing cor-
relations
between
a
given
measure
and
sev-
eral other measures
of
different
reliabilities.
In
this
case,
differences between
the
correla-
tions
that
appear
to be
substantive may,
in
fact,
only
reflect error variance.
Given
the
above concerns,
one
could choose
to
attend
to
only
the
very small number
of
reports
that
contain none
of
these
problems.
This option
was
rejected
in the
present
re-
view
for
four
reasons. First,
the
topic poses
important
questions
that
must
be
answered
by
clinicians
on the
basis
of
existing data.
It
seems
worthwhile, therefore,
to
attend
to
whatever
evidence
is
available, albeit with
appropriate caution toward methodological
limitations. Second, close inspection
of the
data across studies
often
reveals
a
conver-
gence
of
results
from
studies
containing dif-
ferent
flaws. It is
unlikely, though possible,
that
different
flaws
would consistently
lead
to
similar erroneous conclusions. Third, some
of
the
results that
may be
attributable
to
methodological differences
are of
interest
in
their
own
right.
For
example,
the
difference
between
clinic
and
nonclinic
samples
is an
intriguing
one. Finally,
in
those
few
studies
that contain none
of the
three
problems,
marital
and
child problems
are
related.
That
is,
researchers
who
used well-established
measures
while studying
intact,
nonclinic
families
have
found
that
teachers'
ratings
of
children's behavioral adjustment were
re-
lated
to
independently obtained parental
re-
ports
of
discord (Block, Block,
&
Morrison,
1981;
Emery,
Weintraub,
&
Neale,
in
press;
Rutter, 1971; Emery, Note
2).
Similarly,
divorce
and
child adjustment problems have
been
found
to be
related
in
nonclinic samples
where child behavior
was
indexed
by
such
independent measures
as
behavioral obser-
vation,
teacher ratings,
and
peer nomina-
tions
(Hetherington,
1972;
Hetherington,
Cox,
&
Cox, 1978). Thus, although meth-
odology
can
affect
estimates
of the
magni-
tude
of the
relation between marital
and
child
problems,
the
relation
is
nevertheless
a
real
and
important one.
One can be
con-
fident
that
the
results
of
investigations
that
used weaker methods
are not
wholly attrib-
utable
to
methodological third variables.
Pathogenic Dimensions
of
Marital Turmoil
A
necessary
step
in
further specifying
the
covariation between marital
and
child prob-
lems
is to
define marital turmoil
and
child
behavior problems more precisely. This task,
however,
is
extremely
difficult.
Neither
the
field
of
abnormal
child
psychology (Achen-
bach,
1978)
nor the
study
of
marital
rela-
tions (Gurman
&
Kniskern, 1978;
O'Leary
&
Turkewitz, 1978)
has
produced
a
widely
accepted classification system.
The
present
review
takes
a
cautious approach
to
child
adjustment
by
limiting discussions primarily
to two
broad
and
well-accepted
dimensions
of
child disturbance: problems
of
overcontrol
(e.g.,
anxiety,
withdrawal)
and
problems
of
undercontrol
(e.g.,
aggression, conduct dis-
orders;
Achenbach
&
Edelbroch, 1978).
More specific problems,
as
identified
by
var-
ious
investigators,
are
occasionally men-
tioned.
It
also
should
be
made
clear
that
this
review
is
concerned only with children's
so-
cial behavior that
is
considered
by
parents,
teachers,
or
mental health professionals
to
be
maladaptive. Important factors such
as
cognitive development
and
adjustment dur-
ing
adult
life
are not
reviewed here.
A
definition
of
marital turmoil
is
more
difficult
to
construct.
A
movement toward
specificity
has
occurred among divorce
re-
searchers
who
originally
studied
single-par-
ent
families
(grouping
the
divorced, wid-
owed,
and
unmarried together), then focused
on
divorce
in
particular,
and now
increas-
ingly
recognize
the
importance
of
studying
the
conflict between divorced
or
divorcing
parents (Levitin, 1979). Unfortunately,
how
one
best
defines
and
measures
conflict,
in
intact
or in
broken families,
is a
matter
of
controversy.
Three theoretically relevant
as-
pects
of the
definition
are the
process
of
con-
flict
(e.g., hitting, arguing, avoidance),
its
content
(e.g.,
sex, child
rearing,
money),
and
the
length
of
time
it
lasts.
The
ideal system, therefore, would
eval-
uate
families
in
terms
of two
discrete cate-
gories:
family
structure (divorced, separated,
intact)
and
content
of
conflict.
It
would
also
provide
magnitude
estimates
along
two
con-
tinuua
representing
the
length
and the
pro-
cess
(from
openly hostile
to
calm)
of
conflict
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
313
within
each content category.
Except,
how-
ever,
for the
structural
divorced/intact dis-
tinction,
which
is
preserved throughout
the
present review, researchers have neither pre-
cisely
nor
uniformly
measured marital tur-
moil.
This
is
unfortunate.
Particular
content,
process,
and
temporal aspects
of
conflict
ap-
pear
to
have more detrimental
effects
on
children
than
do
other aspects.
Separation
From
a
Parent Versus
Interparental
Conflict
A
critical question
is
whether separation
from
a
parent
per se or the
interparental
conflict
that
is
concomitant with divorce
is
more strongly related
to
child behavior prob-
lems. This distinction
is
particularly relevant
because
it
bears
on
such issues
as
whether
parents should stay together
for
the
chil-
dren's sake.
The
association between divorce
and
behavior problems
in
children
has
been
interpreted
as
evidence
that
separation
per
se has a
direct
and
substantial negative
effect
on
the
child, regardless
of the
circumstances
surrounding
the
separation
(Bowlby,
1973).
This interpretation
has met
with popular
support; thus, beliefs about
the
negative
ef-
fects
of a
broken home
are
widely held.
Recent reviews suggest that this causal
hypothesis
has
failed
to
consider adequately
the
effects
of
interparental conflict (Rutter,
1971,
1979). Current evidence suggests
that
interparental
conflict,
not
separation,
may
be the
principal explanation
for the
associ-
ation
found
between divorce
and
continuing
childhood
problems. Five
different
research
approaches support this conclusion.
First,
several investigators have compared children
from
homes broken
by
divorce
or
separation
with
those from homes broken
by
death.
More
behavior problems were found
in
homes broken
by
divorce, suggesting
that
something
other than separation
per se is
having
a
significant
effect
on
these children
(Douglas, Ross, Hammond,
&
Mulligan,
1966;
Gibson, 1969;
Gregory,
1965).
Sec-
ond,
other researchers have
found
that
chil-
dren
from
broken
but
conflict-free
homes
were less
likely
to
have problems than were
children
from
conflictual,
unbroken homes
(Gibson, 1969; McCord,
J
M
McCord,
W,,
&
Thurber, 1962; Nye, 1957; Power, Ash,
Schoenberg,
&
Sorey,
1974).
Third,
as
noted
throughout this review, children's responses
to
divorce
and
discord share many features.
That
is,
undercontrolled behavior,
sex
dif-
ferences,
and
buffering
effects
are
commonly
found
among
the
children
of
both discord
and
divorce. Fourth, children
of
divorced
parents
who
continue
to
have
conflicts
be-
yond
the
divorce have more problems than
do
children from conflict-free divorces
as
shown both
by
clinical impressions (An-
thony,
1974;
Kelly
&
Wallerstein,
1976)
and
by
research results (Hetherington, Cox,
&
Cox, 1976; Jacobson, 1978; Westman
et
al,
1970). Finally,
one
longitudinal investiga-
tion
found that many
of the
problems evident
in
children
from
broken homes were present
well
before
the
children were separated
from
a
parent
(Lambert,
Essen,
&
Head,
1977).
In
sum,
in
studies
of
clinic
and
nonclinic
groups
that
have used both independent
and
nonindependent
raters, interparental
conflict
has
been associated with behavior problems
in
children whether that
conflict
occurred
in
intact marriages, before
a
divorce,
or
after
a
divorce. Thus, considerable evidence sup-
ports Hetherington's observation that chil-
dren
from
broken
or
intact homes charac-
terized
by
interparental
conflict
are at a
greater risk than
are
children from broken
or
intact homes that
are
relatively harmo-
nious
(Hetherington, Cox,
&
Cox, 1979).
Separation
and
Life-Change
Responses
Although
such
data
argue against attrib-
uting
the
association between divorce
and
child
behavior disorders solely
to
separation
from
a
parent, there
is no
suggestion that
separation
has no
effect
on the
child.
An
"acute
distress
syndrome"
is
commonly found
in
children
on
separation from
a
parent
(Bowlby,
1973; Rutter, 1979). This reaction
follows
a
three-stage process
of
acute upset
followed
by
apathy
or
depression, and,
fi-
nally,
loss
of
interest
in the
parents (Bowlby,
1973)
or
adaptation
to the new
situation
(Rutter, 1972).
Awareness
of the
acute
distress syndrome
is
important
to the
understanding
of
children
from
homes where
a
parent
is
leaving
or has
314
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
recently left. Children's separation responses
in
reaction
to
divorce need
to be
docu-
mented,
as do the
effects
of
variations
in the
treatment
of the
children
of
divorce that
may
affect
separation responses (e.g., visitation
patterns,
different
custody arrangements).
Still, there
are two
further reasons
why the
effects
of
separation
per se
appear
to be
less
important
than
the
results
of the
concomi-
tant conflict. First,
as
suggested
by the
three-stage model
and
according
to the
impressions
of
clinicians
and
researchers,
the
separation
effects appear
to be
time-lim-
ited
(Anthony, 1974; Hetherington, 1979),
whereas
conflict
responses
may be
more
en-
during
(Hetherington
et
al.,
1978). Second,
even
those behavioral changes found
follow-
ing
conflict-free
divorce cannot
be
wholly
attributed
to
separation
per se
(Rutter,
1979).
Significant life changes
occur
con-
comitantly
with divorce
and
further compli-
cate inferences about
the
effects
of
separa-
tion
per se
(Hetherington, 1979; Kurdek,
1981).
These include sometimes dramatic
changes
in
financial
status (Herzog
&
Sudia,
1973)
and
altered discipline practices (Heth-
erington
et
al.,
1976).
In
sum,
it
must
be
recognized that paren-
tal
conflict
does
not
terminate
with
the
mar-
riage,
and it
may,
in
fact,
increase
after
di-
vorce. Thus, much
of the
association
be-
tween divorce
and
ongoing child behavior
problems
may be
explained
by a
frequently
overlooked
third variable, namely, interpar-
ental
conflict.
As
indicated later,
the
"con-
flict
responses" found among
the
children
of
divorce have features
in
common with
the
responses
of
children
from
discordant, intact
marriages.
The two
areas
of
investigation,
however,
are
treated
separately insofar
as
children
can
have
at
least three sets
of re-
actions
to
divorce:
conflict
responses, sepa-
ration
responses,
and
life-change responses
associated with
the new
single-parent family.
Although
the
latter
two
types
of
responses
are
important, children's conflict responses
in
both broken
and
intact
families
are the
present
concern.
Amount
and
Type
of
Conflict
Both
the
amount
and
type
of
interparental
conflict
to
which
the
child
is
exposed would
seem
to be
important determinants
of the
effect
of
that
conflict
on the
child. Conflict
that
is
openly
hostile
exposes
the
child
to
more,
presumably pathogenic, parental
in-
teractions,
as
does
conflict
that
lasts
for
a
long
period
of
time.
The few
studies that
have
investigated these variables
are in
agreement with this conjecture. Rutter
et al.
(1974),
using interview ratings, found
a
stronger relation between child problems
and
unhappy marriages characterized
by
quarrelsomeness than between child prob-
lems
and
unhappy marriages characterized
by
apathy. Similarly, Porter
and
O'Leary
(1980)
found
that
a
self-report
measure
of
open
marital conflict
was a
superior predic-
tor of
problems
in
children when compared
with
a
general index
of
marital satisfaction.
Finally,
in a
study
of the
effects
of
divorce,
Hetherington
(Note
4)
found
that
encap-
sulated conflict
was
more weakly associated
with
child behavior problems than
was
open
hostility.
It
should
be
noted, however,
that
although
these results
are
high
in
face
va-
lidity,
the
first
two
studies were
flawed in
that data were obtained
from
a
single source
(i.e., parents). Further documentation
of the
effects
of
open conflict would therefore
be
useful.
Evidence also supports
the
presumption
of
the
increasingly detrimental effects
of
continuing
conflict. Rutter (1980) studied
children
who
were separated
from
their
homes
at an
early
age as a
result
of
family
discord
or
distress.
He
found that
the
chil-
dren
who
later resided
in
harmonious homes
were
at a
decreased
risk
for
emotional
dis-
turbance when compared with their earlier
status.
On the
other hand, those children
who
stayed
in
homes
characterized
by
con-
flict
continued
to
show problem behavior.
Studies
of
divorce
are
consistent with this
finding.
When
there
is
postdivorce turbu-
lence between parents, children have more
problems
and
more frequently come
to the
attention
of
professionals (Hetherington
et
al.,
1976; Kelly
&
Wallerstein,
1976;
West-
man
et
al.,
1970). Because most children
of
divorce
are
exposed
to
some
unknown
amount
of
predivorce conflict,
these
data suggest
that
the
termination
of
conflict,
as
opposed
to its
continuation
or
exacerbation,
is to the
chil-
dren's benefit.
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
315
The
effects
of
both
the
amount
and
type
of
conflict
merit
more
detailed
investigation
because
all
marriages have some periods
of
conflict.
Further
study
of
these
two
variables
could
provide valuable information
on how
best
to
handle normal interparental dis-
agreements
(in
intact
and in
broken
mar-
riages)
and on
when professional help should
be
sought.
To
summarize,
the
following
can be
con-
cluded about
the
dimensions
of
marital rela-
tions relevant
to
children's adjustment:
The
distinction between divorced
and
intact fam-
ilies
is
important because separation
and
life-change
responses
are
adaptations
that
are
unique
to the
children
of
divorce.
On the
other hand, children
of
both divorce
and
discord
are
likely
to
show
conflict
responses.
In an
assessment
of the
effects
of
interpar-
ental
conflict,
distinctions along hostile-calm
and
temporal continuua
and
across various
content areas appear
to be
important.
Un-
fortunately,
such distinctions have
not
been
made consistently
in
past research.
In
fact,
the
reader should
be
alerted
that, unless oth-
erwise noted,
the
studies
reviewed
below
used
summary measures
of
conflict
or
dis-
cord.
These
consist
either
of
global judg-
ments provided
by a
parent
or an
interviewer
or of
rating scales
that
sum
ratings
of
various
content
and
process aspects
of
marital rela-
tions.
Additional
Parameters
of the
Association
Between
Marital
and
Child Problems
A
review
of the
research literature reveals
additional
variables
that
more precisely
specify
the
relation between marital turmoil
and
child behavior problems.
These
variables
are (a)
form
of the
child's behavioral
re-
sponse,
(b) sex
effects,
(c) age
effects,
(d)
effects
of
having
a
good relationship with
one
parent,
and (e)
effects
of
parental
psycho-
pathology.
Form
of the
Child
Problem
It has
been suggested
that
discord
in
intact
marriages
is
related
to
children's disorders
of
undercontrol
but not to
their
disorders
of
overcontrol
(Rutter, 1971).
Most
of the
early
investigations
of
marital turmoil
and
prob-
lem
children focused
on
relations with
de-
linquency
and
found
a
substantial covaria-
tion
(e.g.,
McCord
&
McCord,
1959). Sub-
sequent investigations, which have included
measures
of
other
child behavior, have pro-
vided
mixed results.
In
studies
of
divorce,
Tuckman
and
Regan
(1966)
found that clinic children
from
homes
broken
by
divorce
or
separation were
apt to
have
conduct problems, whereas children
from
intact families
or
homes broken
by
death
had
more anxiety-related problems.
McDermott
(1968,
1970) reported that con-
duct problems were most prevalent
in his
divorce
sample,
but he
also
found
signs
of
depression
in
these
children.
Whereas
the
above
two
studies were based
on
noninde-
pendent, clinical impressions,
a
study
by
Hetherington
et
al.
(1978)
showed that non-
clinic
children
of
divorce were more depen-
dent, disobedient, aggressive, whining,
de-
manding,
and
unaffectionate
than were chil-
dren
from
intact marriages
as
indexed
by
reliable
observational measures.
In
studies
of
intact marriages,
McCord
et
al.
(1962)
found
that discord
was
related
to
feminine-aggressive behavior, antisocial
be-
havior,
and sex
anxiety
but not to
abnormal
fears
in a
nonclinic sample.
In a
clinic group,
Pemberton
and
Benady (1973) found that
marital
discord
was
related
to
childhood
aggression
and
encopresis
but not to
enu-
resis. Porter
and
O'Leary (1980)
found
mar-
ital discord
to be
related both
to
problems
of
conduct
and to
anxiety
in
clinic children,
but two
similar investigations found signif-
icant
relations
only
for
conduct problems
(Emery
&
O'Leary, 1982; Oltmanns
et
al.,
1977).
Also
in
direct
conflict,
Rutter (1971;
Wolkind
&
Rutter, 1973) found significant
relations
for
conduct problems
but not for
neurotic problems
in
nonclinic samples,
whereas
another British general population
report
found
significant
relations
for
both
conduct
and
neurotic problems
(Whitehead,
1979).
Finally,
in a
study that used exem-
plary
methodology, Block
et al.
(1981)
found
that
a
measure
of
parental disagreement
about
child rearing
was
more strongly
re-
lated
to
teachers' ratings
of
undercontrol
of
impulses than
to
their
ratings
of
overcontrol
of
impulses
in
nonclinic
children
who
were
evaluated
1,
2, and 4
years
after
the
marital
index
was
completed.
316
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
Although
these studies present
a
some-
what
conflicting
pattern
of
results, every
in-
vestigation, whether
of
questionable
or
sound
methodology,
found
marital turmoil
to be
related
to
some
form
of
undercontrolled
be-
havior.
The
results
for
overcontrol were
in-
consistent.
The
fact that
undercontrol
is
typ-
ically rated
more
reliably than overcontrol
(Cantwell,
Russell, Mattison,
&
Will, 1979;
Quay
&
Peterson,
Note
1;
Emery,
Note
2)
may
partially account
for
this
difference.
Still,
it
seems
safe
to
conclude that inter-
parental
conflict—in
broken
and in
intact
families—appears
to be
related more strongly
to
children's problems
of
undercontrol than
of
overcontrol.
Different Effects
on
Boys
and
Girls
Considerable
recent evidence suggests that
marital turmoil
has a
greater
effect
on
boys
than
on
girls from both divorced (Cadoret
&
Cain, 1980;
Hess
&
Camara, 1979; Heth-
erington
et
al,
1978;
McDermott,
1968;
Wallerstein
&
Kelly, 1980)
and
intact, dis-
cordant
marriages
(Block
et
al.,
1981;
Emery
&
O'Leary,
1982; Porter
&
O'Leary, 1980;
Rutter,
1971; Wolkind
&
Rutter,
1973).
Nevertheless,
a
careful examination
of the
literature suggests that
one
must consider
a
number
of
factors when interpreting this
finding.
On the
basis
of
data obtained
in
interviews
with nonclinic families,
Rutter
(1971)
found
that discord
in
intact marriages
was
asso-
ciated with school problems
in
boys
but not
in
girls.
Whitehead
(1979)
questioned
this
result.
Citing Becker's (1964) work docu-
menting
that
parental
punitiveness
was re-
lated
only
to
boys' behavior
in
school
but to
both boys'
and
girls' behavior
at
home,
Whitehead
argued
that
the
relation
with
marital turmoil
was
obscured because girls
inhibit aggression
in
school.
She
presented
data from
a
nonclinic sample showing sig-
nificant
associations between mothers'
re-
ports
of the
marriage
and
both
boys'
and
girls' problems
at
home.
The
results reported
by
other investigators (Emery
&
O'Leary,
1982;
Porter
&
O'Leary,
1980),
however,
are
contrary
to
Whitehead's.
In
samples
of
clinic children, significant relations were
found
between parents' ratings
of
marital
discord
and
their ratings
of
behavior prob-
lems
at
home
for
boys, whereas
no
signifi-
cant
relations
were found
for
girls. Similarly,
Gassner
and
Murray (1969) reported
find-
ing
a
greater
difference
in the
degree
of ob-
served
parental conflict between groups
of
clinic
and
nonclinic boys than between sim-
ilar groups
of
girls.
A
factor that
affects
the sex
difference
more
than
the
setting
may be the
type
of
sample
used.
In
nonclinic samples
of
intact
marriages, problems
in
both boys
and
girls
have been found
to be
related
to
discord
(Block
et
al.,
1981; Whitehead, 1979; Emery,
Note
2),
whereas
in
clinic samples, relations
have
been
found only
for
boys
(Emery
&
O'Leary, 1982; Porter
&
O'Leary, 1980).
When
it is
noted
that
Rutter's
(1971)
non-
clinic
sample
was
preselected
to
contain
a
high proportion
of
disturbed children
and
children
of
parents
with individual psycho-
pathology—thus
making
it
comparable
in
many
ways
to a
clinic
group—this
study, too,
is
consistent with
the
clinic/nonclinic
sex
differences.
How
is
this
pattern
of
results
to be ex-
plained?
An
examination
of how
each
sex
may
respond
to
marital turmoil
may
hold
an
answer.
Two
nonclinic
studies
of
intact
mar-
riages (Block
et
al.,
1981;
Whitehead,
1979)
and
one of
divorce
(Hess
&
Camara, 1979)
found
marital turmoil
to be
related directly
to
measures
of
undercontrol only
for
boys,
whereas
those
weaker
associations
that
were
found
for
girls were with overcontrolled
be-
havior.
(In
fact,
one
study [Block
et
al.,
1981]
found
that
discord
was
inversely
re-
lated
to
undercontrol
in
girls.) Because chil-
dren
are
more likely
to be
referred
to
clinics
because undercontrolled behavior
is
more
readily
identified
as a
problem than
is
over-
controlled behavior
(Ross,
1980),
it may be
the
case
that
boys respond
to
interparental
conflict
in a
more pervasive,
maladaptive
manner
that
is
more likely
to
lead
to a
clinic
referral.
Thus,
it may be
that
there
is a sex
difference
in
response
to
marital
discord,
but
the
difference
is in how and how
much
the
sexes
respond,
not
whether they
do.
A
further
complication
exists
in
inter-
preting
sex
differences among
the
children
of
divorce.
These
differences appear
to be a
result
of at
least
a
two-fold process. First,
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
317
as
in
studies
of
conflict
in
intact
families,
differences
betwen
the
sexes
in
terms
of
con-
flict
responses
are
expected.
In
addition,
there
may be a
second
effect
related
to
being
in
the
custody
of the
opposite-sex parent.
Santrock
and
Warshak
(1979),
in a
study
that
used observational
and
self-report mea-
sures
of a
nonclinic sample,
found
that
both
boys
and
girls
of
divorce
had
more behavior
problems
when
the
opposite-sex parent
had
custody
than when
the
same-sex parent
had
custody.
Because divorced mothers currently
have
child custody over
90% of the
time
(Glick,
1979), boys would
be
expected
to
have more postdivorce adjustment problems
than would girls.
Thus,
it
appears
that
boys
whose parents divorce
and
whose mothers
have
custody have
a
dual risk
for
developing
behavior
problems.
In
sum,
it can be
concluded that marital
turmoil,
in
several studies
of
adequate meth-
odology,
is
more strongly related
to
boys'
than
to
girls' obviously maladaptive behav-
ior.
One
needs
to
consider, however, setting
effects,
sampling,
form
of the
response,
and
custody arrangements
in
interpreting
sex
differences.
Further,
it is
possible
that
the
effects
on
girls
may be
delayed and,
as was
found
in one
study, appear prominently
in
subsequent
relations with
the
opposite
sex
(Hetherington,
1972). Girls
are
likely
to be
just
as
troubled
by
marital turmoil
as
boys
are,
but
they
may
demonstrate their feelings
in
a
manner
that
is
more appropriate
to
their
sex
role, namely,
by
becoming anxious, with-
drawn,
or
perhaps very well behaved.
Child's
Age
Various
arguments suggest that either
older
or
younger children should
be
more
affected
by
marital
turmoil.
For
example,
it
could
be
argued that younger children
are
more susceptible because
of
their greater
dependence
on
their parents. Children
as
young
as 1
year
old
have been shown
to re-
spond
to
interparental
conflict
with upset
and
anger (Cummings,
Zahn-Waxier,
&
Radke-Yarrow,
1981).
On the
other
hand,
older children
are
more sensitive
to
emotion
and
may
feel
pressure
to
become
involved
in
interparental
conflict,
thus making them-
selves more vulnerable
to its
effects.
Although
such speculations
are
common,
surprisingly
little evidence
is
available
on
differential
age
effects.
Two
investigations
of
discord
in
intact marriages
that
controlled
for
age
found
no
major
effect
(Porter
&
O'Leary, 1980;
Rutter,
Graham,
Chadwick,
&
Yule,
1976).
These
studies,
however,
ob-
tained nonindependent
data
and
were lim-
ited
to
children
6
years
of age or
older.
Wallerstein
and
Kelly (1974, 1975; Kelly
&
Wallerstein, 1975, 1976),
in
contrast, sug-
gested
that
divorce
has
unique
effects
on
children
of
different
ages, from
2
years
to
adolescence. Their results were based solely
on
clinical impressions
and
small samples,
however,
and
therefore
cannot
be
viewed
as
reliable. Further, although these investiga-
tors reported subtly
different
effects
at
dif-
ferent
ages,
all age
groups were
found
to be
affected
substantially
by
divorce.
Results
from
studies examining
the
effects
of
a
child's
age at the
time
of
divorce have
been
inconsistent. Findings
that
suggest
more detrimental
effects
of
separations
oc-
curring
before
the age of five or six
(Heth-
erington, 1972), after this
age
(Gibson,
1969;
McCord
et
al,
1962),
and no
effect
of
age at the
time
of
separation (Power
et
al.,
1974), have
all
been reported. Thus,
it
has
not
been demonstrated that
a
child's
age
is
an
important determinant
of the
effects
of
marital turmoil. Nevertheless,
age
contin-
ues to be of
interest
and
merits careful con-
sideration
in
future
investigations.
Good
Parent-Child
Relationships
Some recent empirical investigations have
supported
the
clinical impression that
the
parent-child relationship deteriorates
as a
result
of
marital
turmoil
(Hess
&
Camara,
1979;
Hetherington
et
al.,
1979;
Rutter,
1971).
None
of
these
investigators,
however,
suggest that deterioration
is
inevitable,
and,
in
fact,
it is
argued that
a
1
good relationship
with
one
parent
can
"buffer"
the
child
from
some
of the
negative
effects
of
marital tur-
moil
(Hetherington
et
al.,
1979).
In
a
study
of
intact,
nonclinic families,
Rutter
(1971)
reported
that
a
good relation-
ship with
at
least
one
parent
significantly
reduced
the
likelihood
that boys would
be
judged antisocial
by
their teachers.
It is
318
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
worth
noting, however,
that
boys from dis-
cordant
marriages
still
had
more
problems
than
did
boys
from
happy marriages, despite
the
good parent-child relationship. Further,
a
good
parent-child
relationship
was
strictly
defined
as
consisting
of
both positive warmth
and
lack
of
negative feelings
as
determined
by
parental interview.
Similarly,
only
particularly good
parent-
child
relationships produced
buffering
ef-
fects
in a
study
of
children
of
divorce
(Heth-
erington
et
al.,
1979).
Children's
adjust-
ment,
as
measured
by
multiple methods,
was
better only when, based
on
several parenting
indices,
the
parent-child relationship
was
classified
as
very good. Relationships judged
to be
moderate
had no
buffering
effect.
Fur-
ther,
the
very good relationship
had to be
with
the
mother.
Positive father-child rela-
tions
did not
mitigate
the
detrimental
effects
of
marital turmoil.
In
another nonclinic study
of
divorce,
Hess
and
Camara (1979) also
found
that
measures
of the
quality
of the
parent-child
relationship were important predictors
of
child
adjustment
as
indexed
by
multiple
in-
dependent
measures.
In
fact,
statistical
anal-
ysis
revealed that parent-child relations pre-
dicted
a
greater proportion
of the
variance
in
child adjustment than
did
interparental
conflict.
As
with
Rutter's
(1971)
study, how-
ever, conflict
was
related
to
children's
ad-
justment even when there
was a
good par-
ent-child relationship. Finally,
in
contrast
to
Hetherington's
findings,
good relation-
ships with either parent appeared
to
have
a
buffering
effect.
In
sum,
there
is
emerging evidence based
on
adequate methodology that
a
particularly
warm
relationship with
at
least
one
parent
can
mitigate,
but not
eliminate,
the
effects
of
marital turmoil
on
children. Without
doubt,
the
quality
of
parent-child
relation-
ships
deserves further attention
in
studies
of
both discord
and
divorce.
Individual
Parental
Psychopathology
It is
well documented
that
children from
families
where
a
parent
has a
psychological
disturbance
are at an
increased risk
for a
variety
of
behavioral
problems
(Mednick
&
McNeil, 1968). Further, disordered
in-
dividuals
are
more likely
to
have discordant
marriages
and to
divorce (Molholm
&
Din-
itz, 1972); thus,
it is
possible that marital
turmoil
may
partially explain
the
increased
problems among
the
children
of
these
indi-
viduals.
Data supporting this possibility come
from
Rutter
(1971).
He
found
that
discord
in in-
tact
marriages
was
related
to
antisocial
be-
havior
in the
children both
of
normal parents
and
of
parents with
a
personality disorder.
On the
other hand, when
the
marriage
was
harmonious,
there
was no
increased antiso-
cial behavior associated with parental per-
sonality
disturbance. There was, however,
a
trend toward
an
even greater
risk
for
anti-
social problems when both discord
and
per-
sonality
disturbance were present.
Emery
et al. (in
press) reported similar
findings
among
the
children
of
parents with
other diagnoses.
In
intact marriages discord
explained most
of the
association between
parents'
affective
disorder
(unipolar
depres-
sion
or
bipolar disorder)
and
children's dis-
turbed
school behavior. When
the
effect
of
marital
discord
was
taken
into
account,
little
association
was
found
between these disor-
ders
and
children's behavior.
But
when
the
diagnosis
was
parental
schizophrenia, mar-
ital
discord
did not
explain
the
children's
problems
in
school.
In
fact, little association
was
found
between discord
and
child behav-
ior
problems
in the
schizophrenic
group,
per-
haps
because
the
children
of
schizophrenics
were already disturbed
for
other reasons.
Another possibility
is
that, although
the
global
satisfaction ratings were similar, mar-
ital discord
may be
qualitatively
different
in
schizophrenic families. Conflict
may be re-
duced because
one
partner
is
acknowledged
as
sick.
These
two
reports—both
based
on
non-
clinic
samples, independent raters,
and re-
liable
measures—suggest
that, except
in the
case
of
schizophrenia, concomitant marital
turmoil
may
explain
a
good part
of the in-
creased problems among
the
children
of be-
haviorally
disordered
parents.
Researchers
need
to
examine more closely
the
dual, non-
independent
effects
of
interparental
conflict
and
parental
psychopathology
on
children.
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
319
Mechanisms
By
Which
Marital
Turmoil
Affects
Children
Research
on
marital
and
child problems
has not
generally been guided
by
broad eti-
ological rationales.
Rather,
mini-theories
that
offer
only
a few
predictions
or
make
no
allowance
for
critical evaluation
are
more
common. Because
of
such theoretical
defi-
ciencies, this review evaluates several
hy-
potheses about
how
marital turmoil
may
produce childhood disorders:
(a)
disruption
of
attachment
bonds,
(b)
modeling,
(c) al-
tered discipline practices,
and (d)
other
mod-
els, including stress, taking
on the
symptom,
and
child
effects.
Each
hypothesis
is
evaluated
in
terms
of
the
parameters
of the
relation between mar-
ital
and
child
problems.
Where appropriate,
theoretical assumptions
are
weighed against
the
conclusions
that
(a)
concomitant con-
flict,
not
separation
per se,
appears
to be re-
sponsible
for
many
of
divorce's serious, long-
term
pathogenic effects;
(b)
openly hostile
and
continued
conflict
has a
great
effect;
(c)
the
most prominent behavior disorder
is
a
problem
of
undercontrol;
(d)
boys demon-
strate
a
greater observable response than
do
girls;
(e) age has not
been shown
to
alter
the
child's reaction;
(f) a
good relationship with
at
least
one
parent
can
partially
buffer
the
negative
effects;
and (g)
increased
interpar-
ental
conflict
may
explain many
of the
nega-
tive
effects
found
among children
of
parents
with
individual psychopathology. Other
di-
rect
evidence
is
discussed where
it is
avail-
able
and
appropriate.
Attachment
and
Separation
One of the few
detailed hypotheses about
the
effect
of
marital turmoil
on
children
is
the
maternal deprivation hypothesis
(Bowlby,
1969).
According
to
this account,
the
loss
of
a
figure
to
whom
one is
attached (usually
the
mother) produces
an
instinctive
fear
re-
sponse.
The
survival
value
of the
fear
re-
sponse
accounts
for its
instinctive nature.
Fear
keeps
the
infant
proximal
to the
care-
taker, thus increasing
the
chances
for
sur-
vival.
On the
basis
of
this rationale, Bowlby
(1973) concluded that whenever
an
attach-
ment
figure
is
lost
for any
reason, anxiety
results.
The
specificity
of
this hypothesis makes
it
easily
testable,
but
theoretical assumptions
in
relation
to
marital turmoil
may
have been
overstated.
The
strongest prediction
from
the
attachment hypothesis
is
that
divorce
because
it
involves separation
from
a
par-
ent—has
a
direct,
negative
effect
on the
child.
Although separation responses have
clearly been demonstrated, evidence
re-
viewed
earlier suggests
that
concomitant
in-
terparental conflict, rather than separation
per
se, is the
more salient factor
in
creating
behavior
problems
in the
children
of
divorce.
Further,
the
hypothesis predicts
that
mater-
nal
deprivation
is the
most damaging
be-
cause
the
mother
is
most commonly
the
pri-
mary
attachment
figure.
Divorce usually
re-
sults
in
paternal deprivation, thus
the
hypothesized
effects
on
these
children
are
unclear.
Other parameters discussed earlier
are
also
in
conflict
with
the
attachment hypoth-
esis. Separations before
the age of
five
are
predicted
to
be the
most damaging (Bowlby,
1973),
but no
consistent
effects
of age at the
time
of
divorce were
found.
Problems dem-
onstrated
in the
animal literature
as
result-
ing
from
maternal deprivation
are
frequently
related
to
fears
and
poor
social
development
(Rutter, 1972),
yet
marital turmoil
was
found
to be
most strongly associated
with
conduct disorders. Finally,
sex
differences
are not
explicitly predicted
by the
theory.
Variations
on the
attachment hypothesis
(e.g., Goldstein, Freud,
&
Solnit, 1973) have
suggested that loss
of
love (emotional
de-
privation)
may be the
central cause
of
prob-
lems.
In
support
of
this hypothesis, some
studies comparing clinic
and
nonclinic fam-
ilies
found
that
parents
of
clinic children
were
more negative with their children (Bug-
ental,
Love,
&
Kaswan, 1972; Lobitz
&
Johnson,
1975a; Love
&
Kaswan, 1974).
Further, review
of the
buffering
effect
sug-
gested that
a
good parent-child relationship
may
lessen
the
detrimental
effect
of
marital
turmoil.
Nevertheless,
it is
unclear whether loss
of
love
accounts
for
these
findings
or
whether
some other aspect
of the
parent-child
rela-
322
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
1975).
If
parents disagree about discipline,
boys
should
be
affected more than girls
be-
cause boys
are
disciplined more often
by
both
parents.
For
example, fathers
may
counteract maternal discipline
of
their sons
but not
object
to the
mothers' rules
for
their
daughters.
In
short, sons more frequently
than
daughters
may
become
the
focus
of
in-
terparental
conflict
about discipline.
Direct evidence
on the
relation between
marital turmoil
and
discipline practices
is
rare.
One
sound empirical study
(Hether-
ington
et
al.,
1976) documented changes
in
postdivorce
discipline. When compared with
parents
from
intact marriages, divorced par-
ents were
found
to
make
fewer
maturity
de-
mands, have poorer communication,
be
less
affectionate,
and be
more inconsistent with
their
children. Effects
of the
discipline
changes
were
that
children
of
divorce, espe-
cially
boys, were less compliant with paren-
tal
commands than were children
of
intact
marriages. This latter
finding is
particularly
important
to the
discipline hypothesis
be-
cause
it was
also found that boys were
ex-
posed
to
more
ineffective
discipline than
were girls
(Hetherington
et
al.,
1978).
Changes
in
discipline practices were
greatest
1
year after
the
divorce.
By the
sec-
ond
year, differences between intact
and di-
vorced parents were less noticeable
(Heth-
erington
et
al.,
1976).
It is not
known, how-
ever,
whether
the
changes
in
discipline began
before
or
after
the
divorce. This
is a
very
important
distinction
for
questions
of
etiol-
ogy
and
treatment.
Still,
the
study demon-
strated that discipline
was
disrupted
by
mar-
ital turmoil, although when
the
change
oc-
curred
and
what type
of
change most affected
the
child
are
important questions
that
have
been
left
unanswered.
Supporting
the
possibility
that
discipline
may
change
before
a
divorce,
a
study
of in-
tact
marriages (Block
et
al.,
1981) found
that
an
index
of
parental disagreement about
child rearing
was
related
both
to
subsequent
marital dissolution
and to
future undercon-
trol
in
boys'
and
overcontrol
in
girls' school
behavior.
Inconsistency
in
discipline
was
thus shown
to be
both
an
aspect
of
marital
turmoil
and a
precursor
of
problems
in
chil-
dren. Such prospective
findings
provide
strong support
for the
hypothesis
that
in-
consistent discipline
is a
mediating factor
between marital turmoil
and
child behavior
problems.
As
such,
the
investigation provides
a
model
for
future research.
In
sum, discipline
is an
important
influ-
ence
on
children
and one
that
is
altered
con-
comitantly
with marital problems. Inconsis-
tent discipline,
in
particular, demands con-
tinued attention, according
to the
results
of
two
well-constructed investigations.
Other Hypotheses
Three additional
hypotheses—stress,
tak-
ing
on the
symptom,
and
child-effects
make important contributions
to
under-
standing
how
marital turmoil
may
affect
children.
Because
the
data
on
these
hy-
potheses
are
more limited than that
on the
previous
hypotheses,
their unique implica-
tions
are
presented below with less critical
evaluation
of
each hypothesis
as a
whole.
Stress.
Marital problems
can be
concep-
tualized
simply
as a
stressor
to
which chil-
dren,
as
well
as
adults,
are
differentially
ex-
posed. Marital disruption
has
been noted
to
have
pathogenic
effects
on
adults (Bloom,
Asher,
&
White,
1978),
and it may
similarly
pose problems
for
children
in
that coping
with
life's
difficulties
takes
a
psychological
toll.
A
stress
hypothesis
is of
interest
primarily
in
explaining
the
differential
sex
effects
of
marital turmoil.
The
simplest stress hypoth-
esis would suggest that exposure
to the
stressor
is
different
for
each
sex.
That
is,
girls
may
be
shielded from interparental
conflict
more than
are
boys, Clinical impressions
that
this
is
indeed
the
case
have been
offered
(Wallerstein
&
Kelly, 1980). Research,
however,
has
shown that parents report
fighting
equally
in
front
of
children
of
both
sexes
(Porter
&
O'Leary,
1980)
and
that
boys
and
girls report similar awareness
of
discord
between
their
parents (Emery
&
O'Leary,
1982). Further,
if
differential
ex-
posure accounted
for the sex
effects, mea-
sures
of
open conflict
or of
children's aware-
ness
of
conflict
should
relate
at
least
to
over-
controlled problems
in
girls,
but
this
has not
been
the
case
(Emery
&
O'Leary,
1982;
Porter
&
O'Leary,
1980).
As an
alternative,
the
sexes
may be ex-
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
323
posed equally
to
interparental
conflict,
but
girls
may be
less susceptible
to the
stressor.
One
such explanation
is
that girls
are
less
susceptible
than
boys
to a
variety
of
physical
stressors; therefore, they
may
also have
a
greater resistance
to
psychological stress
(Erne,
1979;
Rutter,
1970).
A
second such explanation
of the sex
dif-
ference
is
suggested
by a
recent investiga-
tion.
Male
and
female college students were
found
to be
equally accurate
in
recognizing
marital
conflict,
but the two
sexes made dif-
ferent
interpretations
of the
conflict. Specif-
ically,
when judging marital interactions
that were seen
as
equally discordant
by
both
sexes, college women rated
the
couple
as
more
"right
for
each
other"
(Epstein,
Fin-
negan,
&
Bythell,
1979). This
suggests
that
sex-role
socialization
may
explain
the
dif-
ferent effect
on
boys
and
girls. Although
both
sexes
are
equally aware
of the
discord,
parental conflict
may be
interpreted
differ-
ently
by
girls,
who may
view
the
same
stressor
as
being
less
stressful.
A
diathesis-stress model
offers
a final
stress
explanation
of the sex
difference.
Base-rate data
on
childhood disorders
in
the
United
States
suggest that boys
are
predis-
posed
toward undercontrol, whereas girls
are
predisposed
toward overcontrol
(Ross,
1980).
Given
this diathesis
and a
marital turmoil
stressor, each
sex may be at
risk
for
devel-
oping
a
disorder. Because undercontrol
is
more easily recognized
and
less tolerated
than
overcontrol, however,
the finding
that
boys
are
more
affected
than girls
by
marital
turmoil
would
be
expected.
Taking
on the
symptom.
The
hypothesis
that
one
member
of a
family becomes
the
focus
of
larger family conflicts also merits
consideration. Minuchin
and his
colleagues
(Minuchin,
1974; Minuchin
et
al.,
1975)
suggested
that
children
serve
the
function
of
distracting their parents
from
their
own
con-
flicts
by
developing
a
problem
themselves
and
thus redirecting parental concerns.
Chil-
dren's defusion
of
interparental conflict
by
"taking
on the
symptom"
is,
therefore,
a
fifth
etiological
hypothesis.
This hypothesis
offers
yet
another expla-
nation
of the sex
differences
and
also
might
explain
some data
on the
form
of the
child's
behavioral
response.
Boys
may
take
on the
symptom
of
interparental
conflict
in the
sex-
role-appropriate manner
of
becoming
ag-
gressive, noncompliant,
and so on.
This very
noticeable disruptiveness
may
indeed dis-
tract
a
boy's parents
from
their
own
prob-
lems.
Undercontrolled behavior
is
more
ob-
vious
than overcontrolled behavior
and
might
facilitate
the
distraction.
But
why
wouldn't girls serve
the
same
function?
Perhaps they
do, but
girls' behav-
ioral distractions might have
a
very
different
topography.
Rather than being disruptive,
girls
may
become anxious, withdrawn,
or
even
very well behaved. Girls
may
actually
refocus
parental attention
by
their extreme
prosocial behavior.
For
girls,
prosocial
and
overcontrolled
behavior
is
more sex-role
ap-
propriate
than
is
undercontrolled behavior,
therefore being nice
or
worried
may
serve
the
distraction
function
better.
This
hypoth-
esis would explain
the
paradox
in the
Block
et al.
(1981)
data
that
showed
that,
for
girls,
parental disagreement
was
related
to
over-
control
in the
expected
direction
but was in-
versely
related
to
undercontrol.
At the
least,
a
taking-on-the-symptom
hypothesis sug-
gests that prosocial
or
competent behavior
be
included
as a
dependent measure
in
stud-
ies of
marital turmoil.
Child-effects,
Instead
of the
assumption
that marital turmoil causes problems
in
chil-
dren,
the
reverse argument could
be
true;
namely,
that
a
deviant child places strain
on
a
marriage. Bell
and
Harper's
(1977)
cogent
arguments
concerning such child-effects need
to be
considered. Indeed,
it has
been
found
that children
can
decrease marital satisfac-
tion
as
indexed
by
such
findings
as (a) the
decline
of
marital satisfaction after
the
birth
of
the first
child,
(b) the
negative
effect
on
a
marriage
as a
result
of
rearing
a
handi-
capped child,
and (c)
survey data wherein
parents report
that
their
children
are an
added stress
on
their marriage (Lerner
&
Spanier,
1978).
It is
likely
that
a
conduct
problem child would similarly strain
a
mar-
riage.
Despite
the
consideration
of a
child-ef-
fects
alternative,
the
causal sequence
from
parent
to
child
is
probably
a
more important
pathway.
One
group
of
researchers
(Olt-
manns
et
al.,
1977)
found
that marital sat-
isfaction
did not
increase
concomitantly
with
324
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
parent-rated improvement
in
problem chil-
dren's
behavior,
a
result contrary
to a
child-
effects
prediction. Further,
from
a
strict
child-effects
position,
it
becomes
difficult
to
explain
why
boys disrupt
a
marriage more
or why
open
conflict
is the
principal result
of
a
child's
problem behavior.
Still,
as
others
have
noted (Lerner
&
Spanier, 1978; Mar-
golin,
in
press),
the
best
explanation
of the
relation
between
the two
areas
of
difficulty
is
reciprocal
influence.
Marital
and
child
problems
are
best viewed
as
interactive:
To
an
extent, each causes
and
exacerbates
the
other.
Summary
It is
unlikely that
any
single hypothesis
fully
explains
the
relation between marital
and
child problems,
yet
each
may
prove
to
have
merit.
Parents
involved
in
conflict with
each other
are
probably poorer models,
are
more
inconsistent
in
their
discipline,
and
place more stress
on
their children. Some
children
probably serve
to
distract attention
away
from
parental
conflict,
whereas others
may
aggravate
the
conflict.
Attachment
bonds
are
certainly disrupted
by
separation
and
may
also
be
affected
by
conflict.
These
(and perhaps
other)
processes
are
likely
to
operate collectively
in
affecting
the
children
of
marital turmoil, although,
in any
given
instance,
one
influence
may
predominate.
When
the
hypotheses
are
compared,
mod-
eling
and
discipline
practices
would
seem
to
hold
particular merit
in
light
of the
available
data.
Still,
because
most
of the
data
have
preceded
specific
predictions, even these
two
etiological accounts cannot
be
given strong
support.
This general lack
of
hypothesis test-
ing
is
characteristic
of the
literature
on
mar-
ital
and
child problems
and
should
not
con-
tinue.
It is
hoped that
the
above delineation
of
various
potential
etiological
mechanisms
will
encourage
future
investigators
to
make
specific
predictions based
on
theoretical
ra-
tionales.
Implications
for
Treatment
Although questions
of
etiology
are far
from
being answered,
at
least
one
implica-
tion
of the
research
on
marital
and
child
problems
appears
to be
clear:
Efforts
should
be
made
to
minimize children's involvement
in
interparental conflict. Although this
may
seem
to be
only common sense, there
are
therapists
who
argue
that
one
must increase
family-wide
conflict
following
a
"civilized"
divorce
as a
means
of
allowing
the
members
to
work through their feelings (e.g., Futter-
man,
1980).
At the
level
of
prevention,
four
suggestions
can be
offered.
First, parents should
work
toward
the
difficult
goal
of
keeping their
children
out of
their angry disagreements,
lest
the
children learn
that
differences
are
resolved
by
yelling,
fighting, or
hitting. Sec-
ond, parents should
attempt
always
to
agree
in
front
of the
child about
at
least
one im-
portant
topic: discipline.
Third,
parents
should
make
a
special
effort
to
maintain
their individual relationship with each child,
as
this
may
partially
buffer
the
child from
the
interparental conflict. Finally, more par-
ents need
to be
aware
that
conflicts between
them
can
have negative
effects
on
their chil-
dren.
Parents need
to be
sensitive
to how
their
children react
to
marital turmoil
and
to be
prepared
to
seek outside help
if
these
reactions
are
prolonged.
At the
treatment level,
families
in
which
marital
and
child problems
coexist
present
particularly
difficult
problems. This
is
docu-
mented
both
by
clinical impressions (Kent
&
O'Leary, 1976; Patterson, Cobb,
&
Ray,
1973)
and by
research that suggests that
treatment
is
less
effective
(Strain,
Young,
&
Horowitz, 1979; Clark
&
Baker, Note
5).
Because
of
this
difficulty
therapists
need
to
be
open
to new
approaches
and
roles.
For
example,
the
suggestion that individually
oriented
interventions
may be
less successful
than
treatment that takes place
on the
fam-
ily
level
(e.g.,
Minuchen,
1974) deserves
consideration.
Behavioral
family
therapy
was
shown
in one
study (Oltmanns
et
al.,
1977)
to be
effective
despite
the
presence
of
marital discord.
As an
alternative, recent
evidence suggests that
the
marriage rather
than
the
entire
family
may be the
more
ap-
propriate initial level
for
treatment
(Mar-
golin
&
Christensen,
Note
6).
Families
with
both marital
and
child problems were found
to
improve more
on
self-report
and
obser-
vational measures
of the
family,
the
mar-
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
325
riage,
and the
child when randomly assigned
to
behavioral marital therapy
as
opposed
to
family
treatment
(Margolin
&
Christensen,
Note
6).
Although
these
data
are
prelimi-
nary,
they
point
out the
importance
of
com-
parisons
of
innovative treatments.
Child
and
family therapists must also
be
willing
to
adopt
other,
nontraditional
ap-
proaches. Parents
who are
divorced
or who
come
from
discordant marriages often
do not
receive
the
spousal
support
that
can
main-
tain
positive
efforts
at
child rearing
(Wahler,
1980).
Therapists, therefore, should consider
group
approaches
to
parent training when
family
and
couple options
are
impossible.
Therapists also need
to
take
a
more
active
role
in
legal decisions
involving
marital
and
child
problems.
For
example, Haynes
(1978)
suggested
a new
role
for
mental health
professionals
as
divorce mediators. Rather
than
resolving decisions such
as
child cus-
tody
and
visitation
via the
adversarial legal
process,
it is
possible that more
of
these
de-
cisions
can be
reached with
the
help
of an
impartial
mediator.
The
latter
process would
seem
to
offer
opportunities
for
reducing con-
flict,
whereas
the
former procedure
is
likely
to
exacerbate
it.
Finally,
therapists need
to
keep
in
touch
with
the
emerging
empirical
literature
and
distinguish
it
from opinion.
For
example,
contrary
to
popular
belief,
the
literature
re-
viewed
here
suggests
that
divorce
may
ulti-
mately
result
in a
less detrimental environ-
ment
for a
child's
emotional development,
at
least
in
homes
where
conflict
is
great
and
where
divorce
will
lead
to a
diminution
of
that
conflict.
Such
an
interpretation might
not
only benefit children from
intact
but
con-
tinually
conflicted
families,
but it
might also
relieve
divorced parents' guilt
over
the ir-
reversible damage they
may
believe they
have caused their children. Further,
the fo-
cus on
interparental
conflict
underscores
both parents' continued responsibility
to im-
prove
relations with
the
former spouse
for
the
children's sake. Nevertheless, despite
the
fact
that divorce
may
sometimes
be the
least
detrimental alternative, evidence
does
sug-
gest
that
divorcing parents should
be
pre-
pared
for
three unique reactions
from
their
children: conflict responses, separation
re-
sponses,
and
life-change responses. Parents
should
also know that, unfortunately, psy-
chologists
do not yet
have
all the
answers
to
the
many questions
on how
marital turmoil
affects
children.
Future Trends
If
answers
to
some
of the
questions
are to
be
forthcoming, much more sound research
will
have
to be
done. Given below
are
some
interpretative
and
methodological
consider-
ations that
will
need
to be
addressed
in ad-
dition
to
specific proposals
for
future
re-
search.
Methodological
Considerations
Because marital
and
child problems
can
be
studied only
in a
correlational design
and
because alternative interpretations
are
theo-
retically
relevant
to the
topic,
reverse cau-
sality
and
third variable interpretations need
to be
considered. Reverse causality
was
dis-
cussed earlier
as the
etiological model
of
child-effects;
only third variables
are
consid-
ered now.
A
variety
of
strategies
are
available
for
investigating
the
effects
of
third variables.
The
important point
is to
recognize those
variables
that
are
potential
mediators.
In the
marital-child area, variables related
to
mar-
ital
turmoil such
as
social class
and
parental
psychopathology
can be
controlled
for in
various
research designs.
Indeed,
it has
been
shown
that these
two
variables
do not
fully
account
for the
observed
relation
(Rutter,
1971;
Rutter
et
al.,
1974), although
the fi-
nancial
decline experienced
by
one-parent
families
clearly
produces
important
life-
change responses
in the
children
(Colletta,
1979;
Herzog
&
Sudia, 1973). Other vari-
ables such
as
postdivorce custody arrange-
ments,
visitation
patterns,
and
social support
networks
also need
to be
measured
and
con-
trolled
as
relevant third variables.
One
third variable mentioned earlier
is
worth
discussing again: response bias
in
non-
independent
reports
of
marital
and
child
problems. Recent investigations suggest
that
parents' reports
of
behavior
problems
in
their
children
can
sometimes
reflect
more
about
the
parent than about
the
child.
In one
study (Griest,
Wells,
&
Forehand, 1979),
326
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
maternal
ratings
of
child adjustment were
more strongly related
to
maternal depression
than
to the
child's
observed
behavior.
It was
argued
that
the
depression
had
colored
the
mothers' perceptions.
It is
possible
that
a
similar bias might
ex-
plain
some
of the
association
found
between
marital
turmoil
and
child behavior problems.
One way to
avoid this problem
is to use in-
dependent
raters
or
observational measures.
Because parents have unique
access
to
many
aspects
of
their children's behavior, however,
it is
important
to
determine whether marital
turmoil
does alter parents' perceptions
of
their
children.
The
comparison
of
parental
report with observational measures
(Griest
et
al.,
1979),
the
assessment
of
rating biases
such
as
social desirability (Robinson
& An-
derson,
Note
3), and the
measurement
of
cognitive
set
toward evaluating children's
behavior
(Messe,
Stollak,
Larson,
& Mi-
chaels,
1979)
are
three methods that
can be
used
in
obtaining data
on
this issue.
In
addition
to
third-variable considera-
tions,
methodological improvements
are
needed.
The
taxonomy
of
child
and
marital
problems
and the
instruments available
to
measure their dimensions need
to be
devel-
oped further.
In the
assessment
of
children,
more reliable measures
of
social competence
and
of
overcontrol
are
especially needed.
With regard
to
marital problems, instru-
ments
are
needed that
will
assess
the
process
and the
length
of
conflict independently
along
different
continuua,
within
discrete
categories
of
conflict
content. While
the
sys-
tems
of
classification
and
measurement
of
child
and
marital problems
are
being
re-
fined,
research should
use at
least
one ac-
cepted measure
of
each problem. Measures
of
adequate reliability
are
available
on the
self-report
of
child adjustment (Achenbach
&
Edelbrock, 1978)
and
marital relations
(Cromwell, Olson,
&
Fournier,
1976)
as
well
as on the
observation
of
child behavior (Pat-
terson,
Ray, Shaw,
&
Cobb, Note
7) and
marital
interaction
(Olson
&
Strauss,
1972;
Hops, Wills, Patterson,
&
Weiss,
Note
8).
The
definition
of the
population
of
interest
is
a final
methodological consideration that
needs
to be
addressed.
Most
of the
investi-
gations
reviewed
above
have studied
special
populations,
usually children referred
for
therapy.
Although
a
relation between mar-
ital
and
child problems
is
found
in
nonclinic
samples (Block
et
al.,
1981; Gibson, 1969;
Rutter
et
al.,
1976), stronger associations
seem
to be
found
within clinic groups
(O'Leary
&
Emery,
in
press; Oltmanns
et
al.,
1977).
The
study
of
nonclinic families
would
not
only provide valuable epidemio-
logical
data
but
would
also control
for two
important confounds.
First,
people
who
seek
treatment
are
self-selected
and
represent
an
ill-defined
subgroup
of the
general popula-
tion.
Second,
the
fact
of
being
the
subject
of
both research
and
treatment
may
have
problematic
reactive
effects. Because
it has
been
demonstrated that
it is
easier
to
fake
bad
(i.e., make
the
family
look more dis-
turbed
than
it is)
than
to
fake good (i.e.,
make
the
family
look healthier than
it is;
Lobitz
&
Johnson, 1975b),
the
possibility
that clinic samples might wish
to
make their
family
and
children look more disturbed
in
order
to
justify treatment
is of
most concern.
Research
Directions
Although suggestions
for
research have
been
made
throughout
the
text,
four specific
research proposals deserve special attention.
First,
a
large,
cross-sectional, multi-pre-
dictor epidemiological study
on a
represen-
tative U.S. sample
is
needed.
The
degree
of
covariation between types
of
marital
turmoil
and
child behavior problems
has not yet
been
documented
in
sufficient
detail. Marital tur-
moil
is
related
to
increased behavioral dis-
turbance
in
children,
but it
cannot
yet be
clearly specified
how
great
the
risk
is. The
value
of
epidemiological data would
be
greatly augmented
by
including moderator
variables such
as the
type, length,
and
con-
tent
of
interparental conflict;
the age and sex
of
children;
and the
presence
of
psycholog-
ical problems
in the
parents,
as
well
as by
including
demographic information. Find-
ings from such
a
cross-sectional
analysis
would
yield valuable data
on
which children
in
what circumstances
are at
greatest risk
because
of a
given type
of
marital problem.
Second,
more descriptive studies using
detailed
and
reliable
self-report
and
obser-
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
327
vational
measures
are
needed. Comparisons
between
(a)
intact
and
broken families char-
acterized
by
interparental
conflict
and (b)
other families
in
terms
of
variables such
as
open conflict, child-rearing practices,
affec-
tion, treatment
of
children
of
different
sexes,
and
child behavior
are
needed.
Data
from
investigations
of
this
nature
could
provide
tests
of
etiological accounts
and
offer
de-
scriptions
that would help
to
develop
a
tax-
onomy
of
marital
and
family relations.
Third,
and
perhaps most
important,
pro-
spective
investigations
are
badly
needed.
Longitudinal
data
can
provide controls
for
reverse causality
as
well
as
yield
descriptive
information
on the
course
of
children's
re-
sponses
to
marital discord
and
divorce. Such
critical issues
as (a) the
distinction
of
sep-
aration, life-change,
and
conflict
responses;
(b) the
timing
of
changes
in
parent-child
interactions resulting from
changes
in
par-
ent-parent interactions;
(c) the
effect
of
con-
flict
of
various types
and
lengths;
and (d) the
child's role
in
aggravating interparental con-
flict can be
properly addressed only through
prospective
investigation.
The
added
expense
involved
in
gathering longitudinal data
is
clearly
offset
by its
quality
and
importance.
Finally,
studies
are
needed
on how
martial
turmoil
affects
the
outcome
of
traditional
treatments
for
children
as
well
as on the ef-
fectiveness
of new
treatments. Alternative
treatments
for
study include
(a)
family ther-
apy
>
(b)
conjoint therapy
for
parents (from
both
intact
and
broken marriages) preceding
treatment
of the
child,
(c)
teaching child
management
to
single parents
in
groups,
and
(d)
divorce mediation
as an
alternative
to
the
legal,
adversarial
resolution
of
custody
and
visitation decisions.
Both
marital
and
child problems
are
com-
mon
in the
United
States
today, Careful
in-
vestigation
of the
relation between
the
prob-
lems
could
have
broad
implications
for the
prevention
and
treatment
of
behavior dis-
orders
in
children
and
could allow profes-
sionals
to
better meet
the
needs
of
both par-
ents
and
children.
At
present,
the
many chil-
dren from homes where interparental conflict
is
a
common occurrence
are a
high-risk pop-
ulation.
As
demonstrated
by
the
relatively
meager psychological
literature,
research
and
treatment directed
at
lowering this risk
have
not
been
of
high priority. Given
the
importance
and the
frequency
of the
prob-
lems, this should
not be
allowed
to
continue.
Reference
Notes
1.
Quay,
H.
C.,
&
Peterson,
D. R.
Manual
for the
Behavior Problem
Checklist.
Unpublished manu-
script,
1979. (Available
from
D. R.
Peterson,
39
North
Fifth
Street,
Highland
Park,
New
Jersey
08904.)
2.
Emery,
R. E.
Marital discord
and
child behavior
problems
in a
nonclinic
sample.
Unpublished
manu-
script, University
of
Virginia,
1981.
3.
Robinson,
E,
A.,
&
Anderson,
L. L.
Family
ad-
justment,
parental attitudes,
and
social desirability.
Paper presented
at the
meeting
of the
American Psy-
chological
Association,
Los
Angeles, August 1981.
4.
Hetherington,
E. M.
Personal
communication,
March
22,
1981.
5.
Clark,
D.
B.,
&
Baker,
B. L.
Training parents
of
developmentally
disabled children: Prediction
of
follow-up outcome.
1.
Paper presented
at the
meet-
ing
of the
American Psychological Association,
New
York,
August 1979.
6.
Margolin,
G., &
Christensen,
A. The
treatment
of
families with marital
and
child
problems.
Paper
presented
at the
meeting
of the
Association
for the
Advancement
of
Behavior
Therapy, Toronto,
No-
vember
1981.
7.
Patterson,
G.
R.,
Ray,
R.
S.,
Shaw,
D.
A.,
&
Cobb,
J. A.
Manual
for
coding
of
family interactions.
Un-
published
manuscript,
University
of
Oregon
and
Oregon
Research
Institute,
1969.
8.
Hops,
H.,
Wills,
T.
A.,
Patterson,
G.
R.,
&
Weiss,
R.
L.
Marital interaction coding
system.
Unpub-
lished
manuscript, University
of
Oregon
and
Oregon
Research
Institute,
1972.
References
Achenbach,
T. W.
Psychopathology
of
childhood:
Re-
search
problems
and
issues. Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical Psychology,
1978,
46,
759-776.
Achenbach,
T.
M.,
&
Edelbroch,
C. S. The
classifica-
tion
of
child
psychopathology:
A
review
and
analysis
of
empirical
efforts.
Psychological
Bulletin,
1978,
55,
1275-1301.
Anthony,
E. J.
Children
at
risk
from
divorce:
A
review.
In
E. J.
Anthony
and C.
Koupernik
(Eds.),
The
child
in
his
family.
III.
New
York: Wiley, 1974.
Armentrout,
J. A.
Parent child-rearing attitudes
and
preadolescents'
problem behaviors.
Journal
of
Con-
sulting
and
Clinical Psychology,
1971,
35,
278-285.
Bandura,
A.
Principles
of
behavior modification.
New
York:
Holt,
Rinehart
&
Winston,
1969.
Bandura,
A.
Aggression:
A
social
learning
approach.
Englewood
Cliffs,
N.J.: Prentice-Hall,
1973.
Baumrind,
D.
Current patterns
of
parental
authority.
Developmental Psychology Monographs,
1971,
4(1,
Pt. 2).
328
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
Becker,
W. C.
Consequences
of
different
kinds
of pa-
rental
discipline.
In M. L.
Hoffman
&
L.
W.
Hoffman
(Eds.),
Review
of
child
development.
New
York:
Russell
Sage
Foundation, 1964.
Becker,
W.
C.,
Peterson,
D. R.,
Hellmer,
L.
A.,
Shoe-
maker,
D. J., &
Quay,
H. C.
Factors
in
parental
behavior
and
personality
as
related
to
problem
be-
havior
in
children. Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical
Psychology, 1959,
23,
107-118.
Becker,
W.
C.,
Peterson,
D.
R.,
Luria,
Z.,
Shoemaker,
D.
J.,
&
Hellmer,
L. A.
Relations
of
factors
derived
from
parent
interview
rating
to
behaviour
problems
of
five-year-olds.
Child Development, 1962,
33,
509-
535.
Bell,
R. Q., &
Harper,
L. V.
Child
effects
on
adults.
Hillsdale,
N.J.:
Erlbaum,
1977.
Block,
J.
H.,
Block,
J.,
&
Morrison,
A.
Parental agree-
ment-disagreement
on
child-rearing
orientations
and
gender-related personality
correlates
in
children. Child
Development, 1981,
52,
965-974.
Bloom,
B.
L.,
Asher,
S.
J.,
&
White,
S. W.
Marital
disruption
as a
stressor:
A
review
and
analysis. Psy-
chological
Bulletin,
1978,
85,
867-894.
Bowlby,
J.
Attachment
and
loss:
I.
Attachment.
New
York:
Basic
Books,
1969.
Bowlby,
J.
Attachment
and
loss:
II.
Separation.
New
York:
Basic Books, 1973.
Bugental,
D.
E.,
Love,
L.
R.,
&
Kaswan,
J. W.
Vid-
eotaped
family
interaction:
Differences
reflecting
presence
and
type
of
child
disturbance.
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology,
1912,79,
285-290.
Cadoret,
R.
J.,
&
Cain,
C. Sex
differences
in
predictors
of
antisocial
behavior
in
adoptees. Archives
of
Gen-
eral
Psychiatry, 1980,
37,
1171-1175.
Cantwell,
D. P.,
Russell,
A. T.,
Mattison,
R.,
&
Will,
L. A
comparison
of DSM II and DSM III in the
diagnosis
of
childhood psychiatric disorders. Archives
of
General
Psychiatry,
1979,
36,
1208-1228.
Chawla,
P.
L.,
&
Gupt,
K. A
comparative
study
of
parents
of
emotionally
disturbed
and
normal
children.
British
Journal
of
Psychiatry, 1979,
134,
406-411.
Cherlin,
A. The
effect
of
children
on
marital dissolution.
Demography, 1977,
14,
265-272.
Colletta,
N. D. The
impact
of
divorce: Father absence
or
poverty?
Journal
of
Divorce, 1979,
3,
27-38.
Cromwell,
R.
E.,
Olson,
D. H.
L.,
&
Fournier,
D. G.
Diagnosis
and
evaluation
in
marital
and
family
coun-
seling.
In D. H. L.
Olson
(Ed.),
Treating relation-
ships.
Lake
Mills,
Iowa:
Graphic,
1976.
Cummings,
E.
M.,
Zahn-Waxler,
C.,
&
Radke-Yarrow,
M.
Young children's responses
to
expressions
of
anger
and
affection
by
others
in the
family.
Child Devel-
opment,
1981,
52,
1274-1282.
Douglas,
J. W.
B.,
Ross,
T.
M.,
Hammond,
W.
A.,
&
Mulligan,
D. G.
Delinquency
and
social
class.
British
Journal
of
Criminology, 1966,
6,
294-302.
Erne,
R. F. Sex
differences
in
childhood psychopathol-
ogy:
A
review.
Psychological Bulletin, 1979,
86,
574-
596.
Emery,
R.
E.,
&
O'Leary,
K. D.
Children's perceptions
of
marital
discord
and
behavior
problems
of
boys
and
girls.
Journal
of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 1982,
10,
11-24.
Emery,
R.
E.,
Weintraub,
S.,
&
Neale,
J. M.
Effects
of
marital
discord
on the
school
behavior
of
children
of
schizophrenic,
affectively
disordered,
and
normal
parents.
Journal
of
Abnormal
Child Psychology,
in
press.
Epstein,
N.,
Finnegan,
D.,
&
Bythell,
D.
Irrational
be-
liefs
and
perceptions
of
marital
conflict.
Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical Psychology, 1979,
67,
608-
609.
Flanders,
J. P. A
review
of
research
on
imitative
be-
havior.
Psychological
Bulletin,
1968,
69,
316-337.
Framo,
J. L.
Personal
reflections
of a
therapist. Journal
of
Marriage
and
Family Counseling, 1975,
/,
15-28.
Futterman,
E. A.
After
the
"civilized"
divorce.
Journal
of
the
American Academy
of
Child Psychiatry,
1980,
19,
525-530.
Gassner,
S.,
&
Murray,
E. J.
Dominance
and
conflict
in
the
interactions
between
parents
of
normal
and
neurotic
children.
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology,
1969,
74,
33-41.
Gibson,
H. B.
Early
delinquency
in
relation
to
broken
homes.
Journal
of
Child Psychology
and
Psychiatry
and
Allied
Disciplines, 1969,
10,
195-204.
Glick,
P. C.
Children
of
divorced
parents
in
demo-
graphic
perspective. Journal
of
Social Issues, 1979,
55,
170-182.
Glick,
P.
C.,
&
Norton,
A. J.
Marrying,
divorcing,
and
living
together
in the
U.S. today.
Population
Bulletin,
1978,
32,
3-38.
Goldstein,
J.,
Freud,
A.,
&
Solnit,
A. J.
Beyond
the
best
interests
of the
child.
New
York: Free Press, 1973.
Gregory,
I.
Anterospective
data
following
childhood
loss
of
a
parent. Archives
of
General
Psychiatry,
1965,
13.
110-120.
Griest,
D.,
Wells,
K.
C.,
&
Forehand,
R.
Examination
of
predictors
of
maternal perceptions
of
maladjust-
ment
in
clinic-referred
children.
Journal
of
Abnormal
Psychology, 1979,
88,
277-281.
Gurman,
A.
S.,
&
Kniskern,
D. P.
Research
on
marital
and
family
therapy: Progress, perspective,
and
pros-
pect.
In S. L.
Garfield
& A. E.
Bergin
(Eds.),
Hand-
book
of
psychotherapy
and
behavior change.
New
York: Wiley, 1978.
Haynes,
J. M.
Divorce mediator:
A new
role. Social
Work,
1978,
23,
5-9.
Herzog,
E.,
&
Sudia,
C. E.
Children
in
fatherless fam-
ilies.
In B. M.
Caldwell
& H. N.
Riccuti
(Eds.),
Re-
view
of
child development research (Vol.
3).
Chicago:
University
of
Chicago Press, 1973.
Hess,
R.
D.,
&
Camara,
K. A.
Post-divorce
family
rela-
tionships
as
mediating factors
in the
consequences
of
divorce
for
children.
Journal
of
Social Issues, 1979,
35,
79-96.
Hetherington,
E. M.
Effects
of
father
absence
on
per-
sonality
development
in
adolescent daughters. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 1972,
7,
313-326.
Hetherington,
E. M.
Divorce:
A
child's perspective.
American Psychologist, 1979,
34,
851-858.
Hetherington,
E.
M.,
Cox,
M.,
&
Cox,
R.
Divorced
fathers.
Family Coordinator, 1976,
25,
417-428.
Hetherington,
E.
M.,
Cox,
M.,
&
Cox,
R. The
aftermath
of
divorce.
In J. H.
Stevens
& M.
Mathews
(Eds.),
Mother/Child Father/Child Relationships. Washing-
ton,
D.C.: National Association
for the
Education
of
Young
Children, 1978.
CHILDREN
OF
DISCORD
AND
DIVORCE
329
Hetherington,
E.
M,
Cox,
M., &
Cox,
R.
Family
in-
teraction
and the
social
emotional
and
cognitive
de-
velopment
of
children
following
divorce.
In V.
Vaughn
&
T.
Brazelton
(Eds.),
The
family:
Setting
priorities.
New
York:
Science
and
Medicine, 1979.
Jacobson,
D. S. The
impact
of
marital separation/di-
vorce
on
children:
II.
Interparental hostility
and
child
adjustment.
Journal
of
Divorce,
1978,
2,
3-20.
Jacobson,
N.
S.,
&
Martin,
B.
Behavioral marriage
therapy:
Current status. Psychological
Bulletin,
1976,
83,
540-556.
Johnson,
S.
M.,
&
Lobitz,
G.
K.
The
personal
and
mar-
ital
adjustment
of
parents
as
related
to
observed child
deviance
and
parenting behaviors.
Journal
of
Abnor-
mal
Child Psychology, 1974,
2,
193-207.
Kelly,
J.
B.,
&
Wallerstein,
J. S. The
effects
of
parental
divorce:
1.
The
experience
of
the
child
in
early latency,
2.
The
experience
of the
child
in
late latency.
Amer-
ican
Journal
of
Orthopsychiatry,
1975,
45,
253-255.
Kelly,
J.
B.,
&
Wallerstein,
J. S. The
effects
of
parental
divorce: Experiences
of the
child
in
early latency.
American
Journal
of'Orthopsychiatry,
197'6,
46, 20-
32.
Kent,
R.
N.,
&
O'Leary,
K. D. A
controlled evaluation
of
behavior modification with conduct problem
chil-
dren.
Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical Psychology,
1976,
44,
586-596.
Kurdek,
L.
A. An
integrative perspective
on
children's
divorce
adjustment.
American
Psychologist,
1981,
36,
856-866.
Lambert,
L.,
Essen,
J.,
&
Head,
J.
Variations
in be-
havior
ratings
of
children
who
have
been
in
care.
Jour-
nal
of
Child Psychology
and
Psychiatry
and
Allied
Disciplines,
1977,
18.
335-346.
Lerner,
R.
M.,
&
Spanier,
G. B.
Child influences
on
marital
and
family
interaction:
A
life-span perspec-
tive.
New
York: Academic Press, 1978.
Levitin,
T. E.
Children
of
divorce.
Journal
of
Social
Issues,
1979,
35,
1-25.
Lobitz,
G.
K.,
&
Johnson,
S. M.
Normal versus deviant
children:
A
multimethod
comparison.
Journal
of
Ab-
normal
Child
Psychology,
1975,
3,
353-374.
(a)
Lobitz,
W. C., &
Johnson,
S. M.
Parental manipulation
of
the
behavior
of
normal
and
deviant
children. Child
Development, 1975,
46,
719-726.
(b)
Locke,
H.
J.,
&
Wallace,
K. M.
Short marital-adjust-
ment
and
prediction
tests:
Their
reliability
and
valid-
ity.
Marriage
and
Family
Living,
1959,
21,
251-255.
Love,
L.
R.,
&
Kaswan,
J. W.
Troubled
children:
Their
families,
schools,
and
treatments.
New
York: Wiley,
1974.
Margolin,
G. The
reciprocal relationship
between
mar-
ital
and
child
problems.
In J. P.
Vincent
(Ed.),
Ad-
vances
in
Family
Intervention,
Assessment,
and
The-
ory. Greenwich,
Conn.:
JAI
Press,
in
press.
Margolin,
G.,
&
Patterson,
G. R.
Differential
conse-
quences provided
by
mothers
and
fathers
for
their
sons
and
daughters. Developmental
Psychology,
1975,
//,
537-538.
McCord,
J.,
McCord,
W.,
&
Thurber,
E.
Some
effects
of
paternal absence
on
male children. Journal
of
Abnormal
and
Social
Psychology,
1962,64,
361-369.
McCord,
W.,
&
McCord,
J.
Origins
of
crime:
A new
evaluation
of
the
Cambridge-Somerville
Youth
Study.
New
York: Columbia University
Press,
1959.
McCord,
W.,
McCord,
J.,
&
Howard,
A,
Familial cor-
relates
of
aggression
in
non-delinquent
male children.
Journal
of
Abnormal
and
Social
Psychology,
1961,
62,
79-83.
McDermott,
J. F.
Parental divorce
in
early childhood.
American
Journal
of
Psychiatry,
1968, 124, 1424-
1432.
McDermott,
J. F.
Divorce
and its
psychiatric sequelae
in
children.
Archives
of
General
Psychiatry,
1970,
23,
421-427.
Mednick,
S.
A.,
&
McNeil,
T. F.
Current methodology
in
research
on the
etiology
of
schizophrenia: Serious
difficulties
which
suggest
the use of the
high-risk
group
method. Psychological
Bulletin,
1968,
70,
681-
693.
Messe,
L.
A.,
Stollak,
G.
E.,
Larson,
R.
W.,
&
Michaels,
G. Y.
Interpersonal
consequences
of
person
percep-
tion
processes
in two
social contexts.
Journal
of
Per-
sonality
and
Social
Psychology, 1979,
37,
369-379.
Minuchin,
S.
Families
and
family
therapy,
Cambridge,
Mass.:
Harvard University Press, 1974.
Minuchin,
S. et
at.
A
conceptual model
of
psychoso-
matic
illness
in
children: Family organization
and
family
therapy.
Archives
of
General Psychiatry,
1975,
32,
1031-1038.
Molholm,
L.
H.,
&
Dinitz,
S.
Female
mental patients
and
their normal controls.
Archives
of
General Psy-
chiatry, 1972,
27,
606-610.
Nye,
F. I.
Child
adjustment
in
broken
and
unhappy
unbroken
homes. Marriage
and
Family
Living,
1957,
19.
356-361.
O'Leary,
K.
D.,
&
Emery,
R. E.
Marital discord
and
child
behavior problems.
In M. D.
Levine
& P.
Satz
(Eds.),
Developmental
variation
and
dysfunction.
New
York: Academic Press,
in
press.
O'Leary,
K.
D.,
&
Turkewitz,
H.
Methodological errors
in
marital
and
child
treatment
research.
Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical Psychology,
1978,
46,
747-
758.
Olson,
D.
H.,
&
Strauss,
M. A. A
diagnostic tool
for
marital
and
family
therapy. Family Coordinator,
1972,
21,
251-258.
Oltmanns,
T.
F.,
Broderick,
J.
E.,
&
O'Leary,
K. D.
Marital adjustment
and the
efficacy
of
behavior ther-
apy
with
children. Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical
Psychology,
1977,
45,
724-729.
Patterson,
G. R.
Accelerating stimuli
for two
classes
of
coercive
behaviors. Journal
of
Abnormal
Child Psy-
chology, 1977,
5,
335-350.
Patterson,
G.
R.,
Cobb,
J.
A.,
&
Ray,
R. S. A
social
engineering
technology
for
retraining
the
families
of
aggressive boys.
In H. E.
Adams
& I. P.
Unikel
(Eds.),
Issues
and
trends
in
behavior therapy. Spring-
field,
111.:
Charles
C
Thomas, 1973.
Patterson,
G.
R.,
&
Reid,
J. B.
Reciprocity
and
coer-
cion:
Two
facets
of
social systems.
In C.
Neuringer
& J.
Michaels
(Eds.),
Behavior modification
in
clin-
ical psychology.
New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1970.
Patterson,
G.
R.,
Weiss,
R.
L.,
&
Hops,
H.
Training
in
marital skills:
Some
problems
and
concepts.
In H.
Leitenberg
(Ed.),
Handbook
of
behavior
modifica-
330
ROBERT
E.
EMERY
lion
and
behavior therapy.
Englewood
Cliffs,
N.J.:
Prentice-Hall,
1976.
Pemberton,
D.
A.,
&
Benady,
D. R.
Consciously
re-
jected
children.
British
Journal
of
Psychiatry, 1973,
123.
575-578.
Porter,
B., &
O'Leary,
K.
D.
Marital
discord
and
child-
hood
behavior
problems. Journal
of
Abnormal
Child
Psychology,
1980,
80,
287-295.
Power,
M. J.,
Ash,
P. M.,
Schoenberg,
E.,
&
Sorey,
E.
C.
Delinquency
and the
family.
British
Journal
of
Social
Work,
1974,
4,
17-38.
Rosenthal,
R.
Experimenter bias
in
behavioral
re-
search.
New
York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts,
1966.
Ross,
A. O.
Psychological disorders
of
children.
New
York:
McGraw-Hill, 1980.
Rutter,
M. Sex
differences
in
response
to
family
stress.
In
E. J.
Anthony
& C.
Koupernik
(Eds.),
The
child
in
his
family.
New
York:
Wiley,
1970.
Rutter,
M.
Parent-child separation: Psychological
ef-
fects
on the
children.
Journal
of
Child Psychology
and
Psychiatry
and
Allied
Disciplines,
1971,12,
233-
260.
Rutter,
M. The
quality
of
mothering: Maternal depri-
vation reassessed.
Harmondsworth,
Middlesex,
En-
gland:
Penguin,
1972.
Rutter,
M.
Maternal
deprivation,
1972-1978:
New
findings,
new
concepts,
new
approaches. Child
De-
velopment, 1979,
50,
283-305.
Rutter,
M.
Protective
factors
in
children's responses
to
stress
and
disadvantage.
In M.
W.
Kent
& J. E.
Rolf
(Eds.),
Primary
prevention
of
psychopathology:
111.
Promoting social competence
and
coping
in
children.
Hanover,
N.H.:
University
Press
of New
England,
1980.
Rutter,
M.,
Graham,
P.,
Chadwick,
O. F.
D.,
&
Yule,
W.
Adolescent turmoil:
Fact
or fiction?
Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry
and
Allied
Disci-
plines,
1976,
17,
35-56.
Rutter,
M.
et
al.
Attainment
and
adjustment
in two
geographical
areas: III. Some factors
accounting
for
area
differences.
British Journal
of
Psychiatry, 1974,
125,
520-533.
Santrock,
J.
W.,
&
Warshak,
R. A.
Father
custody
and
social
development
in
boys
and
girls.
Social Issues,
1979,
35,
112-125.
Schaefer,
E. S.
Children's
reports
of
parental
behavior:
An
inventory.
Child Development,
1965,36,
413-421.
Schwarz,
J. C.
Childhood origins
of
psychopathology.
American Psychologist, 1979,
34,
879-885.
Strain,
P.
S.,
Young,
C.
C.,
&
Horowitz,
J. An ex-
amination
of
child
and
family
demographic
variables
related
to
treatment
outcomes
during
oppositional
child
training.
Behavior Modification, 1979,
3, 61-
76.
Tuckman,
J.,
&
Regan,
R. A.
Intactness
of
home
and
behavioural
problems
in
children.
Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry, 1966,
7,
225-233.
U.S. Census
Bureau.
Statistical
abstract
and the
United
States.
Washington,
D.C.:
Author,
1980.
Wahler,
R. G.
Deviant child behavior
within
the
family:
Developmental
speculations
and
behavior
change
strategies.
In H.
Leitenberg
(Ed.),
Handbook
of be-
havior
modification
and
behavior therapy.
Engle-
wood
Cliffs,
N.J.:
Prentice-Hall,
1976.
Wahler,
R. G. The
insular
mother:
Her
problems
in
parent-child
treatment.
Journal
of
Applied Behavior
Analysis, 1980,
13,
207-219.
Wallerstein,
J.
S.,
&
Kelly,
J. B. The
effects
of
parental
divorce:
The
adolescent experience.
In E. J.
Anthony
& C.
Koupernik
(Eds.),
The
child
in his
family: Chil-
dren
at
psychiatric
risk.
New
York:
Wiley,
1974.
Wallerstein,
J.
S.,
&
Kelly,
J. B. The
effects
of
parental
divorce:
Experiences
of
the
preschool
child.
Journal
of
the
American Academy
of
Child Psychiatry, 1975,
14,
600-616.
Wallerstein,
J.
S.,
&
Kelly,
J. B.
Surviving
the
breakup.
New
York: Basic
Books,
1980.
Westman,
J.
D.,
Cline,
D.
W.,
Swift,
W.
J.,
&
Kramer,
D. A. The
role
of
child
psychiatry
in
divorce.
Archives
of
General Psychiatry, 1970,
23.
416-420.
Whitehead,
L. Sex
differences
in
children's
responses
to
family
stress:
A
re-evaluation.
Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry
and
Allied
Disciplines,
1979,
20,
247-254.
Wolkind,
S.,
&
Rutter,
M.
Children
who
have
been
"in
care"—An
epidemiological
study.
Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry
and
Allied
Disci-
plines, 1973,
14.
97-105.
Received
May
26,
1981
View publication statsView publication stats