Issues in Educational Research, 27(4), 2017 736
!
!
Developing and validating a metacognitive writing
questionnaire for EFL learners
Majid Farahian
Department of ELT, College of Literature and Humanities, Kermanshah Branch, Islamic Azad University, Kermanshah, Iran
In an attempt to develop a metacognitive writing questionnaire, Farahian (2015)
conducted a study which was based on the results obtained from a semi-structured
interview (Maftoon, Birjandi & Farahian, 2014). After running various exploratory factor
analyses (EFA) to validate the questionnaire two general scales of knowledge and
regulation of cognition emerged; however, regarding the subscales of knowledge and
regulation of cognition no clear pattern was found. As such, in the present study a
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was run to refine the scale and construct the final
questionnaire. The findings led to a hypothesised model comprising two factors of
knowledge of cognition and regulation of cognition with ten subcategories represented in
a 36-item questionnaire.
Introduction
Although the historical background of metacognition, as well as self-regulation, can be
traced back to James, Piaget and Vygotsky (Fox & Riconscente, 2008), it was not until the
1970s that the concept was shaped, and the term metacognition was coined. Flavell (1987)
suggested that metacognitive knowledge is “the part of one’s acquired world knowledge
that has to do with cognitive (or perhaps better, psychological) matters” (p. 21). As a
matter of fact, it includes the individual’s perspective of one’s own cognitive abilities, as
well as others.
After the emergence of process-oriented approaches in writing, notably that of Hayes and
Flower (1980), the vital role of metacognition in the writing process has been widely
acknowledged. Various cognitive processes refer to the crucial role of self-regulatory and
decision making processes which improve writing performance. The emphasis on the
critical role of metacognition has been so great that Hayes and Flower argued that “a great
part of the skill in writing is the ability to monitor and direct one’s own composing
process” (p. 39). Hacker et al. (2009), having the same approach, defined writing as applied
metacognition.
Process-oriented theories of writing conceive of writing as a problem solving activity. The
more one is equipped with higher order processing skills, the more he or she will be
capable of acting successfully in problem solving situations. In other words, it can be
concluded that for a recursive goal directed process to function properly, monitoring a
mechanism for “management of topical, rhetorical and strategic knowledge” (Hawkins,
2007, p. 6) is crucial.
The role of metacognition is also emphasised in the post-process approaches to writing
(Hawkins, 2007), which have criticised cognitive process-oriented approaches to writing as
being “overly individualistic, reductive, and de-contextualized" (p.48). Socio-cognitive
Farahian 737
models and genre-based approaches, for example, have such a stance. It should be noted
that genre-based approaches give a pivotal role to metacognitive processes (Yeh, 2014)
which “have as their object knowledge of genre, discourse, and rhetorical aspects of
academic texts” (Negretti & Kuteeva, 2011). These approaches have no choice but to
admit that apart from the interplay of the effect of social context, affect, and cognition,
metacognition has a decisive role in writing.
Metacognition has also found its place in second language studies (e.g., Blasco, 2016;
Gustilo & Magno, 2015). Wenden (1998), argued that metacognitive knowledge “is a
prerequisite for the self-regulation of language learning: it informs planning decisions
taken at the outset of learning and the monitoring processes that regulate completion of a
learning task…” (p. 528). Apart from its role in different language learning skills,
metacognitive knowledge has been recognised as a significant attribute affecting the
process, as well as the product in second language writing (Wang, Spencer & Xing, 2009;
Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994). Research findings show that metacognitive awareness is a
factor which distinguishes poor from skilled writers (Victori, 1999). The metacognitive
growth of second language learners apart from their ethnic, cultural, and linguistic
backgrounds positively correlates with their writing performance (Kasper, 1997).
Metacognition is given even higher credit by some scholars (e.g., Hacker, Keener &
Kircher, 2009) who claim that the writing process from the beginning to the end is an act
of metacognitive behaviour. The reason offered for such an assertion is that the
knowledge of metacognition and its manipulation should be with writers every second
they are involved in the writing.
Parallel to inquiry into the role of the metacognition in learning, a large number of
research studies have shown interest in the measurement of metacognitive knowledge in
second language learning as well. As such, tools for assessing metacognition in second
reading and listening, Metacognitive Awareness Listening Questionnaire (e.g., Vandergrift et al.,
2006) and Metacognitive Awareness of Reading Strategies Inventory (Mokhtari & Reichard, 2002)
were developed; however, despite such a growth of interest in developing measures of
metacognition in second language learning, scant attention has been given to the
development of measures of metacognition in second language/foreign language writing,
though a few studies have dealt with metacognition in second language/foreign language
writing (Kasper 1997; Sperling et al., 2002; Victori, 1999). Research findings identify
metacognitive awareness as a factor which distinguishes poor from skilled writers (Victori,
1999). It has also been found that metacognitive knowledge of second language learners
correlates highly with their writing performance (Kasper, 1997). Metacognition has such
an important role in writing that it has been recognised as an act of metacognitive
behaviour (Hacker et al., 2009). The only study, to the researcher’s best knowledge, which
has attempted to develop a metacognitive knowledge questionnaire on writing, was by
Yanyan (2010), which was based on Flavell’s (1979) model of metacognition including
person, task and strategic knowledge. This turns out to be a limitation of the study as the
framework chosen by Yanyan did not adequately cover the related theoretical assumptions
such as the recent two-dimensional framework of metacognition (e.g., Brown et al., 1983;
Shraw & Dennison, 1994). Besides, there is no report on the validation of the
questionnaire.
738 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
Since measuring metacognition as a general construct for all contexts is very demanding
and may yield inaccurate findings, measures of metacognition have focused on narrower,
domain-specific areas. To this end, the present study, as a follow-up study for Farahian
(2015), aimed to assess the results obtained from factor analysis and refine the scale.
Accordingly, this study addressed the following research question:
Does the MAWQ (Metacognitive Awareness Writing Questionnaire) show good
fit indices as measured by confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)?
Method
As reported in Farahian (2015), although the predicted components formed two general
factors of knowledge and regulation of cognition, the results obtained from exploratory
factor analysis (EFA) did not render reliable factors of MAWQ; therefore, structural
equation modeling (SEM) in AMOS22 (Statistics Solutions, n.d.) was conducted to
investigate the factor structure of the construct. Similar to the steps taken in EFA, first,
the construct validity of the knowledge of cognition was probed. Unlike EFA which did
not allow the researchers to have control over the number of desired factors and their
loading patterns, the CFA begins with a-priori model specified by the researchers and then
tries to support or reject the model.
Participants
The study was conducted in February 2014. The participants were 524 Iranian university
EFL students majoring in different fields of study in English language, including teaching
English, translation, and literature. The participants were selected using convenience
sampling from different universities.
Procedure
As the first step, the participants were interviewed (see Maftoon, Birjandi & Farahian,
2014). A list of statements was generated based on the content analysis of the participants’
responses. Following the inductive data analysis and after the emergence of some
categories, the deductive analysis as the confirmatory stage was adopted. Based on Patton
(2002) “generating theoretical propositions or formal hypotheses after inductively
identifying categories is considered deductive analysis…” (p. 454). At this stage, the
emerged categories were compared and contrasted to the existing categories in the field
(Brown et al., 1983; Schraw & Dennison, 1994; Shraw & Moshman, 1995). The purpose
was to see if the components of metacognitive awareness of the participating Iranian EFL
learners mirrored the literature. As a result, a classification of metacognitive awareness of
Iranian EFL learners emerged (Table 1).
Farahian 739
Table 1: The framework for metacognitive awareness writing knowledge
A: Knowledge of cognition
1. Declarative knowledge (person)
Self-concept and self-efficacy
General facts and opinion
mental translation
the effect of reading in FL
2. Declarative knowledge (task knowledge)
3. Procedural knowledge
4. Conditional knowledge
B: Regulation of cognition
1. Planning and drafting
Audience consideration
2. Monitoring
3. General online strategies
Allocating time and place
Avoidance
Attention
Asking for help
Translation
4. Revision
5. Evaluation
Adapted from Maftoon, Birjandi & Farahian (2014, p. 48).
Following the preparation of the initial item pools they were checked for content validity
by five experts. The resultant list of items was subjected to a pilot test with twenty
participants, who were asked to identify ambiguous items. They were also asked to write
their comments regarding the items. After receiving the feedback the list of statements
was again revised. The questionnaire was translated by a professional translator into
Persian to make sure that the participants’ limited language proficiency in English would
not negatively affect their responses. After the preliminary analyses of reliability and
testing assumptions a list of statements was then developed. To validate the questionnaire,
EFA and CFA were run.
Findings and discussion
As it was reported by Farahian (2015), the reliability indices were acceptable ranging from
.67 to .91. The obtained result from EFA showed a two factor model for metacognitive
awareness. However, no clear pattern emerged regarding the sub-components. Therefore,
it was thought that a CFA may help researcher fine-tune the obtained results.
Trait structures of knowledge of cognition
Figure 1 displays the trait structures of the components of the knowledge of cognition
questionnaire in standardised units. The knowledge of cognition as represented by an
oval at the middle of the diagram has five components each of which has a number of
items which are displayed through smaller sized ovals. It should be noted that three items,
namely, attention, translation, and audience consideration were dropped from the model
because they were the only observed indicators for the latent variables.
740 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
Figure 1: Knowledge of cognition model (standardised estimates)
The model for knowledge of cognition shows that all of the paths between observed and
or latent variables were significant (p < .001), except for GeneKC3 and GeneKC4 (two
items related to general section of knowledge of cognition) which made non-significant
contributions to the model (p > .05).The standardised regression coefficients for the
above mentioned two variables were lower than .30, the minimum acceptable value.
Figure 2 clearly shows the trait structures of knowledge of cognition after removing the
two non-significant observed variables. In the revised model, all the paths between
observed and /or latent variables were significant (p < .001). Moreover, the paths in
standardised units all of the standardised regression coefficients were higher than .30.
The model fit indices showed a good fit for the revised model. However, it is noteworthy
that although the chi-square test was significant (χ
2
(45) = 284.60, p < .05), the large
sample size might have resulted in the significance value. The ratios of the chi-square over
the degrees of freedom (1.72 < 3) also indicated that the model enjoyed a good fit. The
RMSEA value of .033 and its 95 percent confidence intervals (.030 and .045) were all
lower than .05, another indication of the fit of the revised model. The p-close fit value of
.998 (> .05) indicated the knowledge of cognition enjoyed a good fit. The CFI (.97 > .95)
also showed the good fit of the revised model.
Farahian 741
Figure 2: Revised knowledge of cognition model (standardised estimates)
Trait structures of recognition of cognition
Figure 3 shows the trait structures of the components of the recognition of cognition
questionnaire in standardised units. The recognition of cognition, as shown by an oval in
the middle of the diagram, has eight components each of which has a number of items
shown by rectangles.
Unlike the knowledge of cognition model, the recognition of cognition needs a number of
revisions. Based on the results, eight variables were deleted, namely Plan5, Plan7, AsH1,
AsH2, GeneST2, GeneST 4, Eval2 and Monit5, due to their non-significant and / or low
contribution to the model. Unlike the expectation, this model did represent a good fit to
the data since none of the fit indices were at the recommended levels; therefore, the
regulation of cognition model was revised twice. The non-significant paths were deleted
first. The resultant model did not achieve a good fit either. The majority of the fit indices
did not show a good fit. Finally, the Monitoring component of the model was removed to
render the measurement model 4 (see Figure 4). It should be noted that all of the
standardised paths between observed and /or latent variables were significant (p < .001).
742 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
Figure 3: Regulation of cognition model (standardised estimates)
The model fit indices demonstrated that the revised model provided a good fit for the
data and the chi-square test was significant (χ
2
(45) = 284.60, p < .05). The ratios of the
chi-square over the degree of freedom (1.72 < 3) also indicated that the model enjoyed a
good fit. The RMSEA value of .033 and its 95 percent confidence intervals (.030 and .045)
were all lower than .05, another indication that the fit of the model was adequate.
Furthermore, the p-close fit value was .998 (> .05) suggesting a good fit for the knowledge
of cognition.
Farahian 743
Figure 4: Revised regulation of cognition model (standardised estimates)
Trait structures of total model knowledge and regulation of cognition
The combination of the final models of knowledge of cognition (Figures 3 and 4) and
regulation of cognition (Figures 2 and 4) is displayed below in standardised units (Figure
5). The two questionnaires are hypothesised to measure a higher order latent variable, i.e.,
metacognitive awareness of writing (MAW). All the paths connecting the latent and or
observed variables enjoy statistical significance (p < .001).
The MAW model fit indices (Table 2) implied a good fit. As illustrated in Table 1, the
ratios of the chi-square over the degree of freedom (2.08 < 3) indicated a good fit. The
RMSEA value of .046 and its 95 percent confidence intervals (.042 and .049) were all
lower than .05; another evidence for the fit of the model. The p-close fit value of .975 (>
.05) was also indicative of fit of the MAW model.
Table 2: Model fit indices - metacognitive awareness of writing - final revision
Model fit
Chi-square
Ratio of χ
2
over d.f.
GFI
AGFI
RMR
RMSEA
95% CIV RMSEA
p-close for RMSEA
CFI
744 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
Figure 5: Revised model for knowledge of and regulation of cognition
Since the result obtained from EFA did not yield reliable factors, CFA was run to
demonstrate the construct validity of the MAWQ and refined the proposed model. Thus,
as in EFA, first the trait structure of knowledge and regulation of cognition was sought
separately. Later, the trait structure of the whole model was explored. Based on goodness
of fit statistics, the hypothesised models were modified and the items which did not have
Farahian 745
significant observed variables were dropped. Regarding the goodness of fit of the final
model, all model fit indices were satisfactory. Accordingly, a questionnaire (see the
Appendix) with 36 items emerged.
Conclusion
The findings are congruent with the account of metacognition with two general
components (Brown, 1987; Jacobs & Paris, 1987). Although these two components are
interrelated (Brown, 1987; Schraw, 1998), it was found that knowledge and control are
two distinct elements. Moreover, the findings supported the 36 item questionnaire which
can measure metacognitive awareness of Iranian EFL learners.
Additionally, the findings of the study also suggest that three sub-components of
metacognitive knowledge - declarative, procedural, and conditional (Shraw & Moshman,
1995) - well suit Iranian EFL learners; however, based on the findings, apart from
individual’s self-concept reported by Ruan (2014) as a variable affecting person
knowledge, students’ beliefs and opinions regarding the act of composing is part of
person knowledge. Thus, it can be assumed that students’ beliefs with regard to what is
effective writing affects their metacognitive awareness.
Although the results partially supported the literature on metacognition (Schraw &
Dennison 1994; Schraw & Mushman, 1995), removing monitoring and evaluation ran
counter to the expectations, thus, another research study could be conducted to
administer the obtained questionnaire in the same context. As such, future research is
needed to refine the model and identify the nature of the relationships among the factors.
Additionally, further research is needed to interview a number of EFL teachers and seek
their views about the utility of the MAWQ. The follow up study may enquire if they see
the new scale as a tool which provides them with enabling insights into their own
teaching.
The present study makes theoretical and pedagogical contributions to the field of
educational psychology and second language acquisition. First of all, despite the fact that
in recent years few research studies (e.g., Ruan, 2014; Schraw & Dennison, 1994; Sperling
et al., 2002; Vandergrift et al., 2006) have attempted to contribute to a more coherent
picture of the construct of metacognition, to the best of author’s knowledge, no attempt
has been made to present a comprehensive model of metacognitive in foreign language
learning. Thus, the model presented here may contribute to a better understanding of the
nature of metacognition in a domain-specific area as foreign language writing. At the same
time, the presented model may inform research in the area of metacognition, since due to
the abstract nature of metacognitive awareness its operationalisation presents a more
coherent view of the construct. This may contribute to a further consistency in
metacognitive research and at the same time prepare the cornerstone for further
exploration of metacognitive awareness in EFL settings.
746 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
It should be kept in mind that Iranian EFL student have gone through an educational
system in which the general approach toward writing has been predominantly a product-
oriented approach. In addition, instructors have often felt that it was sufficient to provide
students with lexical or grammatical knowledge of the writing task. Such an orientation
has led to the neglect of the process of writing in EFL courses. As a result, many Iranian
EFL learners have failed at acquiring writing skills because they have little or no awareness
of the complexity of writing as a cognitive task. As to pedagogical implications, the
findings may help teachers and students become more familiar with the process of EFL
writing, especially the higher order processes of writing.
It has been argued that metacognition is culture bound and that different educational
environments result in differences in metacognition (Angelova, 2001; Hacker & Boll,
2004). Therefore, while on the one hand selecting participants from one province of Iran
may have resulted in the homogeneity of the sample, on the other hand this reduced the
generalisability of the findings to other EFL contexts. Further research is needed to
randomly select participants and administer the questionnaire in other EFL contexts.
Finally, while providing answers to some questions, and, at the same time, raising new
questions, this study makes a small contribution to the research in the area of
metacognition in EFL writing. Moreover, it generates a new outlook to metacognition in
EFL learning and provides new perspectives for the research on EFL writing. However, it
should not be forgotten that despite its psychometric properties, the MAWQ like any
other scale can be considered as only one source of information regarding EFL students’
metacognitive awareness.
References
Angelova, M. (2001). Metacognitive knowledge in EFL writing. Academic Exchange
Quarterly, 5(3), 78-83. https://www.questia.com/library/journal/1G1-
80679259/metacognitive-knowledge-in-efl-writing-language
Blasco, J. A. (2016). The relationship between writing anxiety, writing self-efficacy, and
Spanish EFL students’ use of metacognitive writing strategies: a case study. Journal of
English Studies, 14, 7-45.
https://publicaciones.unirioja.es/ojs/index.php/jes/article/view/3069
Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more
mysterious mechanisms. In F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and
understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, A. L., Bransford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A. & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning,
remembering, and understanding. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology
(4th ed., Vol. III, pp. 77-166). New York: Wiley.
Farahian, M. (2015). Assessing EFL learners’ writing metacognitive awareness. Journal of
Language and Linguistic Studies, 11(2), 39-51.
http://www.jlls.org/index.php/jlls/article/view/396/218
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-
developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906-911.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
Farahian 747
Flavell, J. H. (1987). Speculation about the nature and development of metacognition. In
F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 21-29).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fox, E. & Riconscente, M. (2008). Metacognition and self-regulation in James, Piaget and
Vygotsky. Educational Psychology Review, 20(4), 373-389.
https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10648-008-9079-2
Gustilo, L. E. & Magno, C. (2015). Explaining L2 writing performance through a chain of
predictors: A SEM approach. 3L: The Southeast Asian Journal of English Language Studies,
21(2), 115-130. http://ejournals.ukm.my/3l/article/view/8787
Hacker, D. J. & Bol, L. (2004). Metacognitive theory: Considering the social-cognitive
influences. In D. M. McInerney & S. van Etten (Eds.), Big theories revisited: Research on
sociocultural influences on motivation and learning (pp. 275-297). Greenwich, CT: Information
Age Publishing.
Hacker, D. J., Keener, M. C. & Kircher, J. C. (2009). Writing is applied metacognition. In
D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education
(pp. 154-172). New York: Routledge.
Hawkins, R. E. (2007). Classifying and characterizing student writers' metacognition: A social
cognitive ethnography. PhD dissertation, Southern Illinois University.
https://search.proquest.com/openview/1198827f27d2ed92cdfe868aa7899990/1?pq-
origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y
Hayes, J. & Flower, L. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L.
Gregg & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3-30). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kasper, L. F. (1997). Assessing the metacognitive growth of ESL student writers. TESL-
EJ, 3(1), 1-20. http://www.tesl-ej.org/wordpress/issues/volume3/ej09/ej09a1/
Maftoon, P., Birjandi, P. & Farahian, M. (2014). Investigating Iranian EFL learners’ writing
metacognitive awareness. International Journal of Research Studies in Education, 3(5), 37-52.
https://doi.org/10.5861/ijrse.2014.896
Mokhtari, K. & Richards, C. A. (2002). Assessing students’ metacognitive awareness of
reading strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 249-259.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.249
Negretti, R. & Kuteeva, M. (2011). Fostering metacognitive genre awareness in L2
academic reading and writing: A case study of pre-service English teachers. Journal of
Second Language Writing, 20(2), 95-110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2011.02.002
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
CA: SAGE.
Ruan, Z. (2014). Metacognitive awareness of EFL student writers in a Chinese ELT
context. Language Awareness, 23(1-2), 76-91.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658416.2013.863901
Schraw, G. (1998). On the development of adult metacognition. In M. C. Smith & T.
Pourchot (Eds.), Adult learning and development: Perspectives from educational psychology (pp.
89-108). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schraw, G. & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 19(4), 460-475. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1994.1033
Schraw, G. & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology Review,
7(4), 351-371. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02212307
748 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
Statistics Solutions (n.d.). AMOS. http://www.statisticssolutions.com/amos/
Vandergrift, L., Goh, C. C. M., Mareschal, C. J. & Tafaghodtari, M. H. (2006). The
metacognitive awareness listening questionnaire: Development and validation. Language
Learning, 56(3), 431-462. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-
9922.2006.00373.x/abstract
Victori, M. (1999). An analysis of writing knowledge in EFL composing: A case study of
two effective and two less effective writers. System, 27(4), 537-555.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0346-251X(99)00049-4
Wenden, A. L. (1998). Metacognitive knowledge and language learning. Applied Linguistics,
19(4), 515-537. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/19.4.515
Yanyan, Z. (2010). Investigating the role of metacognitive knowledge in English writing.
HKBU Papers in Applied Language Studies, 14, 25-46.
http://lc.hkbu.edu.hk/book/pdf/v14_02.pdf
Yeh, H. C. (2014). Facilitating metacognitive processes of academic genre-based writing
using an online writing system. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 28(6), 479-498.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2014.881384
Zimmerman, B. J. & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on writing
course attainment. American Educational Research Journal, 3194), 845-62.
http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.3102/00028312031004845
Appendix: MAWQ (Metacognitive Awareness Writing
Questionnaire)
Items
Strongly
agree
Agree
No idea
Disagree
Strongly
disagree
1.
Writing in English makes me feel bad about
myself.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
2.
I think writing in English is more difficult
than reading, speaking, or listening in
English.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
3.
I believe a successful writer is born not
made.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
4.
Topic familiarity has a significant effect on
one’s writing output.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
5.
A skillful writer is familiar with writing
strategies (e.g., planning or revising the text).
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
6.
At every stage of writing, a skillful writer
avoids making error.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
7.
Dwelling on vocabulary items and grammar
interferes with getting the message across.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
8.
Word by word translation from first
language to English negatively affects one’s
ability in writing.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
9.
I am aware of different types of text types in
writing (e.g., expository, descriptive,
narrative).
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
Farahian 749
10.
I know that the necessary components of an
essay are introduction, body, and conclusion.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
11.
I am familiar with cohesive ties (e.g.,
therefore, as a result, firstly).
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
12.
I know what a coherent piece of writing is.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
13.
I am good at writing topic sentences.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
14.
I know what to do at each stage of writing.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
15.
I find myself applying writing strategies with
little difficulty.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
16.
I know how to develop an appropriate
introduction, body, and conclusion for my
essay.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
17.
I know when to use a writing strategy.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
18.
I know which writing strategy best serves the
purpose I have in my mind.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
19.
I know what to do when the writing
strategies I employ are not effective.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
20.
I know which problem in writing needs
much more attention than others.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
21.
Before I start to write, I prepare an outline.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
22.
I have frequent false starts since I do not
know how to begin.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
23.
Before I start to write, I find myself
visualising what I am going to write.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
24.
My initial planning is restricted to the
language resources (e.g., vocabulary,
grammar, expressions) I need to use in my
essay.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
25.
I set goals and sub-goals before writing (e.g.,
to satisfy the teacher, to be able to write
emails, to be a professional writer).
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
26.
I find myself resorting to fixed sets of
sentences I have in mind instead of creating
novel sentences.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
27.
At every stage of writing, I use my
background knowledge to create the
content.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
28.
I mainly focus on conveying the main
message rather than the details.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
29.
I automatically concentrate on both the
content and the language of the text.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
30.
I can effectively manage the time allocated
to writing.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
31.
I choose the right place and the right time in
order to write.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
32.
I use avoidance strategies (e.g. when I do not
know a certain vocabulary item or structure,
I avoid it).
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
33.
When I cannot write complicated sentences,
I develop other simple ones.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
750 Developing and validating a metacognitive writing questionnaire for EFL learners
34.
After I finish writing, I edit the content of my
paper.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
35.
If I do revision, I do it at the textual features
of the text (e.g., vocabulary, grammar,
spelling).
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
36.
If I do revision, I do it at both textual and
the content levels.
( )
( )
( )
( )
( )
Majid Farahian is an Assistant Professor in the Department of English Language
Teaching, College of Literature and Humanities, Kermanshah Branch, Islamic Azad
University (IAUKSH), Kermanshah, Iran. His main research interests are foreign
language writing, metacognitive awareness in FL writing, and the study of plagiarism in
EFL settings.
Email: farahian@iauksh.ac.ir
Please cite as: Farahian, M. (2017). Developing and validating a metacognitive writing
questionnaire for EFL learners. Issues in Educational Research, 27(4), 736-750.
http://www.iier.org.au/iier27/farahian.pdf